E-ISSN : 2733-4538
This study aimed to examine the schema bias effect on a source monitoring task of negative self-descriptions. Our second purpose was to explore the relationship between source monitoring performance and depression-related variables. A total of 73 university students with higher and lower levels (±1SD) of negative automatic thoughts (ATQ-N) were included. In the first phase, participants were presented the statement that a man always speaks the truth and a woman always speaks falsely. In the second phase, statements intermixed with new self-descriptions were presented on the computer screen, and the participants were to classify each as “new,” “true,” or “false.” The results of this study can be summarized as follows: the group with higher ATQ-N tended to choose the “true” response more often, and had a higher accuracy rate for the “true” items than did the group with lower ATQ-N. Second, the rate of “false” responses was negatively correlated with rumination, while the accuracy rate for “false” items was negatively correlated with belief in automatic negative thoughts (ATQ-NB) in the higher group. The present findings suggest that the automatic processing of source information about maladaptive schema plays an important role in depression-related cognition. Finally, the limitations of this study were discussed along with suggestions for further research.
Ahn, C. K., & Chae, J. H. (1997). Standardization of the Korean Version of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. [Standardization of the Korean Version of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory]. The Korean Journal of Counseling and Psychotherapy, 9, 443-472.
Batchelder, W. H., Hu, X., & Riefer, D. M. (1994). Analysis of a model for source monitoring: Contributions to Mathematical Psychology, Psychometrics, and Methodology. In G. H. Fischer & D. Laming (Ed.) Contributions to Mathematical Psychology, Psychometrics, and Methodology (pp. 51-65). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1990). Multinomial processing models of source monitoring. Psychological Review, 97, 548.
Bayen, U. J., & Kuhlmann, B. G. (2011). Influences of source-item contingency and schematic knowledge on source monitoring tests of the probability-matching account. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(1), 1-17.
Bayen, U. J., Nakamura, G. V., Dupuis, S. E., & Yang, C. L. (2000). The use of schematic knowledge about sources in source monitoring. Memory & Cognition, 28, 480-500.
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings (pp. 1-45). Technical report C-1, the center for research in psychophysiology. Gainesville: University of Florida.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press:
Choi, A. Y., Hwang, S. T., Kim, J. H., Park, K. B., Choi, J. Y., & Hong, S. H. (2014). Validity of the K-WAIS-IV Short Forms. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 413-428.
Chon, K. K., Choi, S. C., & Yang, B., C. (2001). Integrated Adaptation of CES - D in Korea. Korean Journal of Health Psychology, 6, 59-76.
Christianson, S. Å., & Loftus, E. F. (1991). Remembering emotional events: The fate of detailed information. Cognition & Emotion, 5, 81-108.
Cook, G. I., Hicks, J. L., & Marsh, R. L. (2007). Source monitoring is not always enhanced for valenced material. Memory & Cognition, 35, 222-230.
Cook, G. I., Marsh, R. L., & Hicks, J. L. (2003). Halo and devil effects demonstrate valenced-based influences on source-monitoring decisions. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 257-278.
D’argembeau, A. D., Comblain, C., & Linden, M. (2005). Affective valence and the self‐reference effect: Influence of retrieval conditions. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 457-466.
D’argembeau, A. D., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering pride and shame: Self-enhancement and the phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Memory, 16, 538-547.
Degl’Innocenti, A., & Bäckman, L. (1999). Source memory in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorder, 54(1), 205-209.
Denny, E. B., & Hunt, R. R. (1992). Affective valence and memory in depression: Dissociation of recall and fragment completion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 575.
Dobbins, I. G., & Han, S. (2006). Cue-versus probe-dependent prefrontal cortex activity during contextual remembering. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1439-1452.
Dobson, K. S. (Ed.). (2009). Handbook of cognitive-behavioral therapies. New York: Guilford Press.
Doerksen, S., & Shimamura, A. P. (2001). Source memory enhancement for emotional words. Emotion, 1, 5.
Durbin, K. A., Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2017). Source memory that encoding was self-referential: The influence of stimulus characteristics. Memory, 25:1191-1200.
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. (1972). A theory of objective selfconsciousness. New York: Academic.
El Haj, M., & Allain, P. (2012). What do we know about the relationship between source monitoring deficits and executive dysfunction? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 22, 449-472.
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S. J., Lepage, M., Graham, S. J., Grady, C., Keightley, M. L., & Mayberg, H. (2004). Distributed self in episodic memory: neural correlates of successful retrieval of selfencoded positive and negative personality traits. Neuroimage, 22, 1596-1604.
Gotlib, I. H., Kasch, K. L., Traill, S., Joormann, J., Arnow, B. A., & Johnson, S. L. (2004). Coherence and specificity of informationprocessing biases in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 386.
Gutchess, A. H., Kensinger, E. A., Yoon, C., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Ageing and the self-reference effect in memory. Memory, 15, 822-837.
Hayes, S. C. (2005). Get out of your mind and into your life: The new acceptance and commitment therapy. Oakland: New Harbinger Publications.
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test: Manual, revised and expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (PAR).
Hess, T. M., Popham, L. E., Dennis, P. A., & Emery, L. (2013). Information content moderates positivity and negativity biases in memory. Psychology and Aging, 28, 853.
Hicks, J. L., & Cockman, D. W. (2003). The effect of general knowledge on source memory and decision processes. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(3), 489-501.
Higgans, E. T. (1999). When do self-discrepancies have specific relations to emotions? The second-generation question of Tangney, Niedenthal, Covert, and Barlow (1998). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1313-1317.
Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 383-395.
Hwang, S. T., Kim, J. H., Park, K. B., Choi, J. Y., & Hong, S. H. (2012). Standardization of the K-WAIS-IV. Korean Journal of Psychology, 1, 140-140.
Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., & McElree, B. D. (1999). The role of cognitive control: Early selection versus late correction. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Ed.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 383-400). New York: Guilford Press.
Johnson, M. K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 24, 403-416.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3.
Kahan, T. L., & Johnson, M. K. (1992). Self effects in memory for person information. Social Cognition, 10, 30-50.
Keenan, J. M., Golding, J. M., & Brown, P. (1992). Factors controlling the advantage of self-reference over other-reference. Social Cognition, 10, 79-94.
Kim, K., & Johnson, M. K. (2010). Extended self: Medial prefrontal activity during transient association of self and objects. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 199-207.
Kim, S. J., Kim, J. H., & Youn, S. C. (2010). Validation of the Korean-Ruminative Response Scale (K-RRS). Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29, 1-19.
Klein, S. B. (2012). Self, memory, and the self-reference effect: An examination of conceptual and methodological issues. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 283-300.
Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding:The contributions of elaborative and organizational processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 5.
Klein, S. B., & Nelson, C. R. (2014). The effects of self-reference on memory: A conceptual and methodological review of inferences warranted by the self-reference effect. (Ed. T. J. Perfect & D. S. Lindsay). The Sage Handbook of Applied Memory. (Ch. 15)Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kwon, S. M., & Yoon, H. K. (1994). The development study of Korean Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire. Student Rearch, 12, 10-25.
Lee, J. D., & Lee, H. J. (2017). An overview of source monitoring:Concept, assessment, and schema bias. Korean Journal of Psychology:General, 36, 397-420.
Leshikar, E. D., & Duarte, A. (2012). Medial prefrontal cortex supports source memory accuracy for self-referenced items. Social Neuroscience, 7, 126-145.
Leshikar, E. D., & Duarte, A. (2014). Medial prefrontal cortex supports source memory for self-referenced materials in young and older adults. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 236-252.
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2004). Detection theory: A user's guide. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Elbaum.
Marsh, R. L., Cook, G. I., & Hicks, J. L. (2006). Gender and orientation stereotypes bias sourcemonitoring attributions. Memory, 14, 148-160.
Mather, M. (2006). Why memories may become more positive as people age. In B. Uttl, N. Ohta, & A. L. Siegenthaler (Eds.), Memory and emotion: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 135-158). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 33-52.
Mather, M., Mitchell, K. J., Raye, C. L., Novak, D. L., Greene, E. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2006). Emotional arousal can impair feature binding in working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 614-625.
Moon, H. M. (2006). Development and validation of the program for facilitation of psychological acceptance based on acceptance &commitment therapy model. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation), The Catholic University of Seoul, Korea.
Nolde, S. F., Johnson, M. K., & D’esposito, M. (1998). Left prefrontal activation during episodic remembering: An event‐related fMRI study. NeuroReport, 9, 3509-3514.
Nolen-Hoeksema., S. (1991). Response styles questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript.
Pauly, K., Finkelmeyer, A., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). The neural correlates of positive self-evaluation and self-related memory. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 878-886.
Pyczsinski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and thedepressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122-138.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401.
Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. En D. Reisberg, & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 3-40):Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2005). A comparison of attentional biases and memory biases in women with social phobia and major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 62.
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677.
Rybash, J. M., & Colilla, J. L. (1994). Source memory deficits and frontal lobe functioning in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 67-73.
Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (2000). On the self-protective nature of inconsistency-negativity management: Using the person memory paradigm to examine self-referent memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 906.
Serbun, S. J., Shih, J. Y., & Gutchess, A. H. (2011). Memory for details with self-referencing. Memory, 19, 1004-1014.
Siedlecki, K. L., Salthouse, T. A., & Berish, D. E. (2005). Is there anything special about the aging of source memory? Psychology and Aging, 20, 19.
Spaniol, J., & Bayen, U. J. (2002). When is schematic knowledge used in source monitoring? Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 28, 631-651.
Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 121, 371.
Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Psychological treatments for depression:How do they work? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 157-165.
Victor, T. A., Furey, M. L., Fromm, S. J., Öhman, A., & Drevets, W. C. (2010). Relationship between amygdala responses to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depressive disorder. Archives of general psychiatry, 67, 1128-1138.
Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, H., Rose, G., & Macleod, A. K. (1996). The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Memory & Cognition, 24, 116-125.
Yang, J. W., Hong, S. D., Jung, Y. S., & Kim, J. H. (2005). The validation study of Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ-N) and the Positive Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ-P) in adolescents. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 631-646.
Yang, L., Truong, L., Fuss, S., & Bislimovic, S. (2012). The effects of ageing and divided attention on the self-reference effect in emotional memory: Spontaneous or effortful mnemonic benefits? Memory, 20, 596-607.
Yoshimura, S., Ueda, K., Suzuki, S. I., Onoda, K., Okamoto, Y., & Yamawaki, S. (2009). Self-referential processing of negative stimuli within the ventral anterior cingulate gyrus and right amygdala. Brain and Cognition, 69, 218-225.
Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy:A practitioner's guide: New York: Guilford Press.