바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

logo

메뉴

The Effects of Cognitive Load on Craving Triggered by Food Cues in External Eaters

Abstract

This study examined the effects of cognitive load on attentional bias toward food cues in external eaters (EEs) through an eye-movement analysis. As EEs are easily driven to eat upon exposure to food cues, the present study examined the effects of cognitive load on the response toward food cues in EEs based on the elaborated intrusion theory. A total of 65 undergradu ates who have high (n= 33) or low (n= 32) levels of external eating were assigned to either high or low cognitive load condi tions and performed a cognitive load task. While they performed the load task, their implicit craving was measured with an eye-tracking device to examine the levels of attentional bias toward food cues during the task. After the task was completed, explicit craving was investigated through a questionnaire. The results showed that high EEs under a high cognitive load con dition showed lower implicit craving than under a low load condition while their explicit craving did not differ. This study suggests that cognitive load that induces cognitive distraction may lower implicit craving in high EEs.

keywords
external eating, cognitive load, the elaborated intrusion theory, visuo-spatial working memory, eye-tracking device

Reference

1.

Andrade, J., May, J., & Kavanagh, D. (2012). Sensory imagery in craving: From cognitive psychology to new treatments for addiction. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 127-145.

2.

Andres, M., Michaux, N., & Pesenti, M. (2012). Common substrate for mental arithmetic and finger representation in the parietal cortex. Neuroimage, 62, 1520-1528.

3.

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252-1265.

4.

Blechert, J., Meule, A., Busch, N. A., & Ohla, K. (2014). Food-pics:An image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 617.

5.

Burton, P., Smit, H. J., & Lightowler, H. J. (2007). The influence of restrained and external eating patterns on overeating. Appetite, 49, 191-197.

6.

Chaput, J. P., Gilbert, J. A., Gregersen, N. T., Pedersen, S. D., & Sjödin, A. M. (2010). Comparison of 150-mm versus 100-mm visual analogue scales in free living adult subjects. Appetite, 54, 583-586.

7.

Chong, R. K., Mills, B., Dailey, L., Lane, E., Smith, S., & Lee, K. H. (2010). Specific interference between a cognitive task and sensory organization for stance balance control in healthy young adults:Visuospatial effects. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2709-2718.

8.

Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. A. (1976). Fennema-Sherman mathematics attitudes scales: Instruments designed to measure attitudes toward the learning of mathematics by females and males. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 7, 324-326.

9.

Field, M., Mogg, K., Zetteler, J., & Bradley, B. P. (2004). Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: The roles of initial orienting and maintained attention. Psychopharmacology, 176, 88-93.

10.

Forestell, C. A., Lau, P., Gyurovski, I. I., Dickter, C. L., & Haque, S. S. (2012). Attentional biases to foods: The effects of caloric content and cognitive restraint. Appetite, 59, 748-754.

11.

Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). Late positive potential to appetitive stimuli and local attentional bias. Emotion, 10, 441-446.

12.

Gormally, J., Black, S., Daston, S., & Rardin, D. (1982). The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addictive Behaviors, 7, 47-55.

13.

Grand, S. (1968). Color-word interference: An investigation of the role of vocal conflict and hunger in associative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 31-40.

14.

Heeren, A., Philippot, P., & Koster, E. H. (2015). Impact of the temporal stability of preexistent attentional bias for threat on its alteration through attention bias modification. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 49, 69-75.

15.

Hou, R., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Moss-Morris, R., Peveler, R., &Roefs, A. (2011). External eating, impulsivity and attentional bias to food cues. Appetite, 56, 424-427.

16.

Imbo, I., & LeFevre, J. A. (2010). The role of phonological and visual working memory in complex arithmetic for Chinese-and Canadian-educated adults. Memory & Cognition, 38, 176-185.

17.

Kakoschke, N., Kemps, E., & Tiggemann, M. (2015). Combined effects of cognitive bias for food cues and poor inhibitory control on unhealthy food intake. Appetite, 87, 358-364.

18.

Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M., Martin, R., & Elliott, M. (2013). Implicit approach–avoidance associations for craved food cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19, 30-38.

19.

Kim, H., Lee, Y., & Kim, J. (1996). A Study of the reliability and validity of the Korean version of the Eating Behavior Questionnaire. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 15, 141-150.

20.

Larsen, H., Engels, R. C., Wiers, R. W., Granic, I., & Spijkerman, R. (2012). Implicit and explicit alcohol cognitions and observed alcohol consumption: Three studies in (semi) naturalistic drinking settings. Addiction, 107, 1420-1428.

21.

Lee, K. M., & Kang, S. Y. (2002). Arithmetic operation and working memory: Differential suppression in dual tasks. Cognition, 83, B63-B68.

22.

Lee, S. (2012). Development of the Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale for high school and college students. The Korean Journal of Counselling and Psychotherapy, 24, 573-594.

23.

MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 107-123.

24.

Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2014). Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1514-1526.

25.

May, J., Andrade, J., Panabokke, N., & Kavanagh, D. (2004). Images of desire: Cognitive models of craving. Memory, 12, 447-461.

26.

May, J., Kavanagh, D. J., & Andrade, J. (2015). The elaborated intrusion theory of desire: A 10-year retrospective and implications for addiction treatments. Addictive Behaviors, 44, 29-34.

27.

Mitchison, D., & Hay, P. J. (2014). The epidemiology of eating disorders:Genetic, environmental, and societal factors. Clinical Epidemiology, 6, 89-97.

28.

Müller, N., Baumeister, S., Dziobek, I., Banaschewski, T., & Poustka, L. (2016). Validation of the movie for the assessment of social cognition in adolescents with ASD: Fixation duration and pupil dilation as Predictors of Performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2831-2844.

29.

Nijs, I. M., Muris, P., Euser, A. S., & Franken, I. H. (2010). Differences in attention to food and food intake between overweight/obese and normal-weight females under conditions of hunger and satiety. Appetite, 54, 243-254.

30.

Nijs, I. M. T., Franken, I. H. A., & Muris, P. (2009). Enhanced processing of food-related pictures in female external eaters. Appetite, 53, 376-383.

31.

Park, S., & Park, A. (2008). The Effects of the K-Nectar Program Using Reality Therapy on the Body Image and Binge Eating Habits of Female College Students. The Korean Journal of Counselling and Psychotherapy, 20, 43-64.

32.

Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala:From a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 773-783.

33.

Sayette, M. A. (2016). The role of craving in substance use disorders:Theoretical and methodological issues. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12, 407-433.

34.

Sharbanee, J. M., Stritzke, W. G., Jamalludin, M. E., & Wiers, R. W. (2014). Approach-alcohol action tendencies can be inhibited by cognitive load. Psychopharmacology, 231, 967-975.

35.

Skorka-Brown, J., Andrade, J., & May, J. (2014). Playing ‘Tetris’ reduces the strength, frequency and vividness of naturally occurring cravings. Appetite, 76, 161-165.

36.

Tiggemann, M., Kemps, E., & Parnell, J. (2010). The selective impact of chocolate craving on visuospatial working memory. Appetite, 55, 44-48.

37.

Van Dillen, L. F., & Andrade, J. (2016). Derailing the streetcar named desire. Cognitive distractions reduce individual differences in cravings and unhealthy snacking in response to palatable food. Appetite, 96, 102-110.

38.

Van Dillen, L. F., Papies, E. K., & Hofmann, W. (2013). Turning a blind eye to temptation: How cognitive load can facilitate selfregulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 427-443.

39.

Van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E., Bergers, G., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295-315.

40.

Van Strien, T., Herman, C. P., Anschutz, D. J., Engels, R. C., & de Weerth, C. (2012). Moderation of distress-induced eating by emotional eating scores. Appetite, 58, 277-284.

41.

Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11, 249-254.

42.

Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2000). Don't mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 753-763.

43.

Werthmann, J., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Jansen, A. (2011). Can (not) take my eyes off it: Attention bias for food in overweight participants. Health Psychology, 30, 561-569.

44.

Wiers, R. W., & Stacy, A. W. (2006). Implicit cognition and addiction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 292-296.

45.

Witcomb, G. L. (2005). Effects of cognitive distraction on the regulation of human eating behaviour (Doctoral dissertation). Loughborough University, London, UK.

logo