open access
메뉴E-ISSN : 2733-4538
It has been suggested that obsessions tend to induce negative inferences about the self. However, few if any studies have examined the relationship between the type of obsession and negative self-inferences. The present study aims to examine which types of obsession are more likely to induce negative self-inferences. 40 participants were assigned into two groups (i.e., the autogenous obsession group vs. the reactive obsession group). The two groups undertook a thought-suppression task that was designed to make the participants experience their most disturbing mental intrusions. We measured the negative self-evaluations and negative self-conscious emotions of the participants to assess the extent to which their mental intrusions induce negative self-inferences. Participants with autogenous obsessions evaluated themselves more negatively in terms of morality, dangerousness, and competence dimensions of the self, and they reported more negative self-conscious emotions after they completed the task, whereas those with reactive obsessions did not. These results suggest that the extent to which mental intrusions provoke negative self-inferences varies according to the type of obsession and that autogenous obsessions are more likely than reactive obsessions to induce negative self-inferences.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Text Revision 4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Baldwin, M. W. (1997). Relational Schemas as a Source of If-Then Self-inference Procedures. Review of General Psychology, 1(4), 326-335.
Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Epstein, N., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An Inventory for Measuring Clinical Anxiety - Psychometric Properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893-897.
Besiroglu, L., Sozen, M., Ozbebit, O., Avcu, S., Selvi, Y., Bora, A., et al. (2011). The involvement of distinct neural systems in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder with autogenous and reactive obsessions. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 124(2), 141-151.
Belloch, A., Morillo, C., & García-Soriano, G. (2007). Obsessive themes, evaluative appraisals, and thought control strategies: testing the autogenous-reactive model of obsessions. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 7(1), 5-20.
Bloch, M. H., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., Rosario, M. C., Pittenger, C., & Leckman, J. F. (2008). Meta-Analysis of the Symptom Structure of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(12), 1532-1542.
Cho, M. J., Kim, K. H. (1998). Use of the center for epidemiologic studies depression (ces-d) scale in Korea. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186(5), 304-310.
Doron, G., Kyrios, M., & Moulding, R. (2007). Sensitive domains of self-concept in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): Further evidence for a multidimensional model of OCD. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 433-444.
Ferrier, S., & Brewin, C. R. (2005). Feared identity and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(10), 1363-1374.
Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., et al. (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14(4), 485-496.
Gardner, D. G., & Cummings, L. L. (1998). Single-item versus multiple-item measurement scales: An empirical comparison. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58(6), 898-915.
Gibbs, N. A. (1996). Non-clinical populations in research in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16(8), 729-773.
Hajcak, G., Huppert, J. D., Simons, R. F., & Foa, E. B. (2004). Psychometric properties of the OCI-R in a college sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(1), 115-123.
Kwon, S. (1992). Differential roles of dysfunctional attitudes and automatic thoughts in depression: An integrated model of depression. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Queensland, Australia.
Leary, M. R. (2007). Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 317-344.
Lee, H. J., & Kwon, S. M. (2003). Two different types of obsession: autogenous obsessions and reactive obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(1), 11-29.
Lee, H. J., Kim, Z. S., & Kwon, S. M. (2005). Thought disorder in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(4), 401-413.
Lee, H. J., Kwon, S. M., Kwon, J. S., & Telch, M. J. (2005). Testing the autogenous- reactive model of obsessions. Depression and Anxiety, 21(3), 118-129.
Lee, H. J., Lee, S. H., Kim, H. S., Kwon, S. M., & Telch, M. J. (2005). A comparison of auto genous/reactive obsessions and worry in a nonclinical population: a test of the continuum hypothesis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(8), 999-1010.
Lee, H. J., & Telch, M. J. (2005). Autogenous /reactive obsessions and their relationship with OCD symptoms and schizotypal personality features. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(7), 793-805.
Lee, H.-J., & Telch, M. J. (2010). Differences in latent inhibition as a function of the autogenous-reactive OCD subtype. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(7), 571-579.
Lee, H. J., Yost, B. P., & Telch, M. J. (2009). Differential performance on the go/no-go task as a function of the autogenous-reactive taxonomy of obsessions: Findings from a non-treatment seeking sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(4), 294-300.
Lodge, M. (1981). Magnitude scaling: quantitative measurement of opinions. Beverly Hills; London: Sage.
Mataix-Cols, D., Rosario-Campos, M. C., & Leckman, J. F. (2005). A multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2), 228-238.
Morillo, C., Belloch, A., & Garcia-Soriano, G. (2007). Clinical obsessions in obsessive- compulsive patients and obsession-relevant intrusive thoughts in non-clinical, depressed and anxious subjects: where are the differences? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(6), 1319-1333.
Moulding, R., Kyrios, M., Doron, G., & Nedeljkovic, M. (2007). Autogenous and reactive obsessions: Further evidence for a two-factor model of obsessions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21(5), 677-690.
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., van den Hout, M., & de Jong, P. (1992). Suppression of emotional and neutral material. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30(6), 639-642.
O'Connor, K., Aardema, F., & Pelissier. (2005). Beyond reasonable doubt: Reasoning processes in OCD and related disorders. New York: Wiley.
Olatunji, B. O., Williams, B. J., Haslam, N., Abramowitz, J. S., & Tolin, D. F. (2008). The latent structure of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a taxometric study. Depression and Anxiety, 25(11), 956-968.
Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (1993). Obsessive Intrusive Thoughts in Nonclinical Subjects .1. Content and Relation with Depressive, Anxious and Obsessional Symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(8), 713-720.
Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (1999). Metacognition and obsessions. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 6(2), 102-110.
Purdon, C. (2004). Empirical investigations of thought suppression in OCD. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35(2), 121-136.
Rachman, S. (1993). Obsessions, responsibility and guilt. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 149-154.
Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(9), 793-802.
Rachman, S., & de Silva, P. (1977). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16(4), 233-248.
Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied psychological measurement, 1(3), 385-401.
Radomsky, A. S., & Taylor, S. (2005). Subtyping OCD: prospects and problems. Behavior Therapy, 36(4), 371-379.
Rassin, E., Merckelbach, H., Muris, P., & Spaan, V. (1999). Thought-action fusion as a causal factor in the development of intrusions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(3), 231-237.
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 934-960.
Rowa, K., Purdon, C., Summerfeldt, L. J., & Antony, M. M. (2005). Why are some obsessions more upsetting than others? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1453-1465.
Shafran, R., Thordarson, D. S., & Rachman, S. (1996). Thought-action fusion in obsessive- compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10(5), 379-391.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 571-583.
Salkovskis, P. M., & Campbell, P. (1994). Thought Suppression Induces Intrusion in Naturally-Occurring Negative Intrusive Thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(1), 1-8.
Smith, M., Calam, R., & Bolton, C. (2009). Psychological factors linked to self-reported depression symptoms in late adolescence. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 37(1), 73-85.
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York; London: Guilford Press.
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical Effects of Thought Suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5-13.
Woo, C. W., Kwon, S. M., Lim, Y. J., & Shin, M. S. (2010). The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R): psychometric properties of the Korean version and the order, gender, and cultural effects. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(3), 220-227.
Yook, S. P., & Kim, Z. S. (1997). A clinical study on the Korean version of Beck Anxiety Inventory: comparative study of patient and non-patient. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 185-197.