open access
메뉴
E-ISSN : 2733-4538
본 연구의 목적은 한국인을 대상으로 연령, 교육수준, 성별과 기억기능의 관련성을 살펴보는 것이었다. 이를 위해 16-69세의 건강한 한국인 736명에게 언어기억검사인 Auditory Verbal Learning Test와 도형기억검사인 Complex Figure Test를 실시하였다. 자료 분석에서는 인구통계학적 변인간의 혼입을 통제하기 위하여 공변량분석을 사용하였고, 기억기능의 영역과 과정에 따른 차별성을 살펴보기 위해 짝점수 분석을 시행하였다. 주요 결과는 다음과 같았다. 첫째, 연령에 따른 차이는 세 변인들 중 가장 현저하였으며 연령이 높을수록 기억기능이 낮았다. 연령에 따른 기억기능 약화는 언어기억 중에서 시간지연이 긴 조건이나, 인출책략이 필요한 조건에서 더 현저하였다. 둘째, 교육수준에 따른 차이는 연령 다음으로 현저하였으며 교육수준이 높을수록 기억기능이 높았다. 교육수준과 기억기능의 연관성은 언어기억보다는 도형기억에서 더 뚜렷하였고, 도형기억 중에서는 인출책략이 필요한 조건에서 더 뚜렷하였다. 셋째, 성별에 따른 차이는 언어기억에 한정되었으며 여성이 남성보다 우세하였다. 언어기억 중에서는 인출책략이 필요한 조건에서 여성의 우세가 더 현저하였다. 결론적으로, 한국인의 기억기능은 연령, 교육수준, 성별에 따른 차이가 있으며, 이러한 차이는 기억기능의 영역과 과정에 따라 차별화되는 부분이 있다. 본 연구의 주요 의의는 청소년 초기에서 노년 초기에 이르는 대규모의 한국인 표집에서 기억기능을 분석한 점이다. 또한 측정치별 단순 비교에 그치지 않고 여러 짝점수를 구성하여 차별성을 분석한 점도 중요한 의의가 있다.
The aim of the current study was to investigate whether and how memory functions of Koreans are related to their age, education, and gender. To this end, we tested a large number of healthy Korean participants (N=736) aged 16-69 years using two memory scales. One was a verbal memory scale called Auditory Verbal Learning Test and the other was a visuospatial memory scale called Complex Figure Test. The obtained data were subject to multiple analyses of covariance and multiple paired-scores analyses. The results indicated three main findings. First, age accounted for the largest variance in memory scores. The effects of age on verbal memory scores were greater in the condition associated with longer delay and the condition associated with strategic retrieval. Second, education accounted for the second largest variance. The effects of education were greater in visuospatial than verbal memory domain and, within visuospatial domain, greater in the condition associated with strategic retrieval. Third, the effects of gender were restricted to verbal memory scores and indicated superiority of females over males. This gender effect was greater in the condition associated with strategic retrieval. In conclusion, memory functions of Koreans are related to their age, education and gender and these relationships differ, in part, as a function of which memory domain or process is measured. A main value of the current study lies in use of a large Korean sample that includes adolescents, adults, and older adults. Another main value lies in the construction of several paired scores and their systematic analyses.
김홍근 (2013). Rey-Kim 기억검사-II: 해설서. 대구: 도서출판 신경심리.
김홍근, 김용숙, 김태유 (2011). 한국 노인의 연령, 교육수준, 성별이 인지기능 측정치들에 미치는 효과. 한국심리학회지: 임상, 30, 681-701.
서은현, 이동영, 추일한, 윤종철, 김기웅, 우종인 (2007). 벤톤 시각 기억 검사(Benton Visual Retention Test)의 한국 노인 정상규준 연구. 한국심리학회지: 임상, 26, 745- 763.
안효정, 최진영 (2004). 노인용 이야기 회상 검사의 표준화 연구. 한국심리학회지: 임상, 23, 435-454.
이현수 (2005). 노인의 기억은 정말로 떨어지는가? 한국심리학회지: 임상, 24, 581-598.
조미혜, 강연욱 (2010). 한국판 간편 시공간 기억 검사(Korean-Brief Visuospatial Memory Test)의 노인 대상 표준화 연구. 한국심리학회지: 임상, 29, 427-439.
최진영, 이지은, 김명진, 김호영 (2006). 노인용 언어 학습 검사(Elderly Verbal Learning Test)의 개발 및 표준화 연구. 한국심리학회지: 일반, 25, 141-173.
통계청 (2011). 2010 인구주택총조사 전수집계 결과(인구부문) 보도자료. 2011년 5월 30일.
Bleecker, M. L., Bolla-Wilson, K., Agnew, J., & Meyers, D. A. (1988). Age-related sex differences in verbal memory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 403-411.
Bolla-Wilson, K., & Bleecker, M. L. (1986). Influence of verbal intelligence, sex, age, and education on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 203-211.
Bolla, K. I., Lindgren, K. N., Bonaccorsy, C., & Bleecker, M. L. (1990). Predictors of verbal fluency (FAS) in the healthy elderly. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 623-628.
Boone, K. B., Lesser, I. M., Hill-Gutierrez, E., Berman, N. G., & D'elia, L. F. (1993). Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure performance in healthy, older adults: Relationship to age, education, sex, and IQ. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 22-28.
Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44, 195-208.
Caffarra, P., Vezzadini, G., Dieci, F., Zonato, F., & Venneri, A. (2002). Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: Normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurological Sciences, 22, 443-447.
Cohn, M., Emrich, S. M., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Age-related deficits in associative memory: the influence of impaired strategic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 23, 93-103.
Delbecq-Derouesné, J., & Beauvois, M.-F. (1989). Memory processes and aging: A defect of automatic rather than controlled processes? Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 1, 121-150.
Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 335-341.
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959-964.
Elias, M. F., Elias, P. K., D'Agostino, R. B., Silbershatz, H., & Wolf, P. A. (1997). Role of age, education, and gender on cognitive performance in the Framingham Heart Study: Community-based norms. Experimental Aging Research, 23, 201-235.
Fastenau, P. S., Denburg, N. L., & Hufford, B. J. (1999). Adult norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and for supplemental recognition and matching trials from the Extended Complex Figure Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 30-47.
Filley, C. M. (2011). Neurobehavioral anatomy (3rd ed.). Boulder, Colorado: The University Press of Colorado.
Geffen, G., Moar, K., O'hanlon, A., Clark, C., & Geffen, L. (1990). Performance measures of 16- to 86-year-old males and females on the auditory verbal learning test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 4, 45-63.
Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for education. American Psychologist, 52, 1091-1102.
Hartman, M., & Potter, G. (1998). Sources of age differences on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 513-524.
Heaton, R. K., Ryan, L., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1996). Demographic influences on neuropsychological test performance. In I. Grant & K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (2nd ed., pp. 141-163). New York: Oxford University Press.
Herlitz, A., Airaksinen, E., & Nordström, E. (1999). Sex differences in episodic memory: The impact of verbal and visuospatial ability. Neuropsychology, 13, 590-597.
Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2008). Sex differences in episodic memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 52-56.
Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., Kritchevsky, M., & Squire, L. R. (1989). Cognitive impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 548-560.
Kramer, J. H., Delis, D. C., & Daniel, M. (1988). Sex differences in verbal learning. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 907-915.
Levine, T. R., & Hullett, C. R. (2002). Eta squared, partial eta squared, and misreporting of effect size in communication research. Human Communication Research, 28, 612-625.
Lewin, C., Wolgers, G., & Herlitz, A. (2001). Sex differences favoring women in verbal but not in visuospatial episodic memory. Neuropsychology, 15, 165-173.
Miatton, M., Wolters, M., Lannoo, E., & Vingerhoets, G. (2004). Updated and extended Flemish normative data of commonly used neuropsychological tests. Psychologica Belgica, 44, 189-216.
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Brav, T. K., & Levy, O. (2007). The associative memory deficit of older adults: the role of strategy utilization. Psychology and Aging, 22, 202-208.
Parkin, A. J., & Leng, N. R. (1993). Neuropsychology of the amnesic syndrome. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rey, A. (1941). L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encéphalopathie traumatique. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286-340.
Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Press Universitaire de France.
Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1991). Effects of age, education, and gender on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 370-376.
Smith, G., & Rush, B. K. (2006). Normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. In D. K. Attix & K. A. Welsh-Bohmer (Eds.), Geriatric neuropsychology: assessment and intervention. (pp. 27-55). New York: The Guilford Press.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Tombaugh, T. N., Faulkner, P., & Hubley, A. M. (1992). Effects of age on the Rey-Osterrieth and Taylor complex figures: Test-retest data using an intentional learning paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 647-661.
Uchiyama, C. L., D'Elia, L. F., Dellinger, A. M., Becker, J. T., Selnes, O. A., Wesch, J. E., ... Miller, E. N. (1995). Alternate forms of the Auditory-Verbal Learning Test: Issues of test comparability, longitudinal reliability, and moderating demographic variables. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10, 133-145.
Van Der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P., Van Breukelen, G. J., & Jolles, J. (2005). Rey's verbal learning test: normative data for 1855 healthy participants aged 24-81 years and the influence of age, sex, education, and mode of presentation. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 290-302.
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1995). Frontal lobe damage produces episodic memory impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 525-536.