ISSN : 1229-8778
The purpose of this study is to understand the importance of economic factors in order to live a happy life after retirement. Korea is suggesting the necessity of joining the individual pension as a means of economic factors. However, in spite of the previous studies that affect individual pension entrants, the participation rate of private pensions is still low. Therefore, this study considers the individual pension as inter-temporal choice context and suggests the importance of the time factor to increase the intention of intention in the individual pension. Time perception in this study refers to how closely you perceive the time between your current and pension benefits, or whether you are aware that it will be coming soon. The purpose of study 1 was to investigate the importance of time perception in private pensions. In Study 2, proceeding with a factorial design of 2 (prediction: forecasting vs. backcasting) x 2 (affective valence of events: positive vs. negative) to examine the effects of prediction, affective valence of events on purchase intention of individual pension and to confirm that time perception mediates these effects. The result of Study 1 showed that time perception is the most important explanatory factor for joined the individual pension. In Study 2, the main effects of the prediction and the interaction between prediction and affective valence of events on purchase intention of individual pension were statistically significant. Concretely, the purchase intention of individual pensions was higher than that in backcasting condition. Specifically, in forecasting condition, when participants were in anticipated negative events, the purchase intention of individual pension was higher than in anticipation of positive events. In addition, it was statistically significant that time perception moderated mediation effects. These studies have academic and practical significance by examining the influence of practical variables to promote the purchase intention of individual pension.
강현옥, & 황혜원 (2009). 직업활동 참여노인의 생활만족도에 영향을 미치는 요인에 관한 연구. 노인복지연구, 44(단일호), 89-109.
구지연, & 차경욱 (2015). 가계특성에 따른 개인연금 가입여부와 가입유형 비교. 소비자정책교육연구, 11(1), 97-120.
김도환 (2001). 노인이 지각한 가족지지, 자아존중감 및 우울과의 관계 연구. 노인복지연구, 13(단일호), 113-144.
김수완, 김순옥 (2007). 우리나라 다층노후소득보장체계의 구축 전망-사적연금의 수급자수 전망을 중심으로. 사회보장연구, 23(2), 271-295.
김영숙, & 서경현 (2002). 생활환경에 따른 한국노인들의 생활만족과 우울수준. 노인복지연구, 18, 273-290.
김재호 (2013). 개인연금 가입결정과 유지요인에 관한 분석-기업의 개인연금보험료지원 효과를 중심으로. 보험금융연구, 73(단일호), 3-29.
김정애, 김재휘 (2014). 미래 사건의 제시방식과 사건들의 특성이 자기개발 목표행동에 미치는 효과. 한국광고홍보학보, 16(3), 64- 94.
김희연, 김재휘 (2013). 미래에 대한 예측 방식과 자기 조절 모드가 연금 보험 가입 의도에 미치는 효과. 소비자학연구, 24(3), 51-73.
나혜림, 최현자 (2014). 개별연금 가입여부 및 다층연금체계 가입구조의 결정요인 분석. Financial Planning Review, 7(4), 43- 72.
노태석, 남인우 (2016). 고객의 이용 전 마음가짐과 대기 전념물의 콘텐츠가 지각된 대기시간에 미치는 영향. 마케팅관리연구, 21(4), 1-22.
문용필 (2012). 중고령자의 개인연금 가입에 영향을 미치는 요인: 패널자료를 활용한 분석. 노인복지연구, 58(단일호), 89-110.
백은영 (2012). 개인연금 가입의향에 영향을 미치는 요인 분석. 사회보장연구, 28(3), 63-86.
부수현, 김재휘 (2013). 의사결정 시점에 따른 메시지 적합성이 예방행동 의도에 미치는 효과. 광고학연구, 24(1), 7-30.
오진호, 임두빈, & 전용일 (2013). 연금연구: 사적연금 적립금 추이전망. 연금연구, 3(1), 95-111.
여윤경, 이남희 (2012). 개인연금자산의 수요와 적정성에 관한 분석. 보험금융연구, 68(단일호), 63-93.
윤명숙, 이묘숙 (2012). 노인의 경제상태가 행복에 미치는 영향. 한국노년학, 32, 397- 413.
이용하 (2011). 다층연금체계 구축: 내실화 방안을 중심으로. 연금연구, 1(2), 1-27.
이윤호 (2012). 개인연금수요의 구조분석. 대한경영학회지, 25(1), 195-210.
이찬희, 정홍주 (2013). 개인연금 가입과 선호의 결정요인분석: 인지요인, 정서요인, 금융이해력, 신뢰, 위험감수성향을 중심으로. 금융연구, 27(4), 25-51.
전승훈, 임병인, 강성호 (2006). 개인연금 가입 결정 및 가입상태 변화 분석. 보험개발연구, 17(1), 137-168.
정운영, 박주영 (2015). 심리적 특성과 보험관련행동. 소비문화연구, 18(1), 179-196.
조혜진, 최현자, 박주영 (2014). 금융소비자의 자산포트폴리오에 있어서 객관적 위험과 주관적 위험 비교 및 유형화 연구. 소비자학연구, 25(1), 173-198.
주소현 (2011). 개인연금 보유 및 연금불입액의 관련요인 분석. 소비자학연구, 22(3), 183-206.
차경욱 (2015). 개인연금 가입결정과 납입액 규모 및 가입의향에 영향을 미치는 요인. Financial Planning Review, 8(2), 89-122.
통계청 (2015). 2015년 고령자통계, 2015. 09. 24. 보도자료.
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2000). When does duration matter in judgment and decision making?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(4), 508-523.
Baker, J., & Cameron, M. (1996). The effects of the service environment on affect and consumer perception of waiting time: An integrative review and research propositions. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 24(4), 338-349.
Bashir, Y., Wilson, E., Lockwood, P., Chasteen, L., & Alisat, S. (2014). The Time for Action is Now: Subjective Temporal Proximity Enhances Pursuit of Remote-Future Goals. Social Cognition, 32(1), 83-93.
Benzion, U., Rapoport, A., & Yagil, J. (1989). Discount rates inferred from decisions: An experimental study. Management science, 35(3), 270-284.
Carver, S., & Scheier, F. (2000). On the structure of behavioral self-regulation. Handbook of self-regulation, 41-84.
Cohen, B., & Andrade, B. (2004). Affective intuition and task-contingent affect regulation. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(2), 358-367.
Conti, R. (2001). Time flies: Investigating the connection between intrinsic motivation and the experience of time. Journal of personality, 69(1), 1-26.
DeVoe, E., & Pfeffer, J. (2011). Time is tight: how higher economic value of time increases feelings of time pressure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 665-676.
Ebert, E., Gilbert, T., & Wilson, D. (2009). Forecasting and backcasting: predicting the impact of events on the future. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(3), 353-366.
Festjens, A., & Janiszewski, C. (2015). The value of time. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(2), 178-195.
Finucane, L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of behavioral decision making, 13(1), 1-17.
Förster, J., Friedman, S., Özelsel, A., & Denzler, M. (2006). Enactment of approach and avoidance behavior influences the scope of perceptual and conceptual attention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 133-146.
Fredrickson, L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of personality and social psychology, 65(1), 45-55.
Gable, A., & Poole, D. (2012). Time flies when you’re having approach-motivated fun effects of motivational intensity on time perception. Psychological science, 23(8), 879-886.
Garretson, R. C., & Mauser, F. F. (1963). The future challenges marketing. Harvard Business Review, 41, 168-78.
Gilbert, T., Pinel, C., Wilson, D., Blumberg, J., & Wheatley, P. (1998). Immune neglect: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of personality and social psychology, 75(3), 617-638.
Gino, F., & Mogilner, C. (2014). Time, money, and morality. Psychological Science, 25(2), 414-421.
Green, L., Fristoe, N., & Myerson, J. (1994). Temporal discounting and preference reversals in choice between delayed outcomes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(3), 383-389.
Holmberg, J., & Robèrt, H. (2000). Backcasting-A framework for strategic planning. International Journal of Sustainable Development & World Ecology, 7(4), 291-308.
Jacoby, J., Szybillo, J., & Berning, K. (1976). Time and consumer behavior: An interdisciplinary overview. Journal of Consumer Research, 2(4), 320-339.
Kim, K., & Zauberman, G. (2009). Perception of anticipatory time in temporal discounting. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology and Economics, 2(2), 91-101.
Lam, C., & Buehler, R. (2009). Trips down memory lane: Recall direction affects the subjective distance of past events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(2), 230-242.
Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of personality and social psychology, 75(1), 5-18
Monga, A., & Bagchi, R. (2012). Years, months, and days versus 1, 12, and 365: The influence of units versus numbers. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(1), 185-198.
Peetz, J., & Wilson, E. (2013). The post-birthday world: Consequences of temporal landmarks for temporal self-appraisal and motivation. Journal of personality and social psychology, 104(2), 249-266.
Peetz, J., Wilson, E., & Strahan, J. (2009). So far away: The role of subjective temporal distance to future goals in motivation and behavior. Social Cognition, 27(4), 475-495.
Preacher, J., & Hayes, F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers, 36(4), 717- 731.
Rhodes, R., & Strain, J. (2008). Affective forecasting and its implications for medical ethics. Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics, 17(1), 54-65.
Ruby, B., Dunn, W., Perrino, A., Gillis, R., & Viel, S. (2011). The invisible benefits of exercise. Health Psychology, 30(1), 67-74.
Sackett, A. M., Meyvis, T., Nelson, L. D., Converse, B. A., & Sackett, A. L. (2010). You’re having fun when time flies: The hedonic consequences of subjective time progression. Psychological Science, 21(1), 111- 117.
Tice, M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it!. Journal of personality and social psychology, 80(1), 53-67.
Trommsdorff, G. (1983). Future orientation and socialization. International Journal of Psychology, 18(1-4), 381-406.
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2000). Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. Journal of personality and social psychology, 79(6), 876-889.
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological review, 110(3), 403-421.
Wang, J., Hong, J., & Zhou, R. (2017). How long did I wait? The effect of construal levels on consumers’ wait duration judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(1), 169-184.
Wittmann, M., & Paulus, P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in cognitive sciences, 12(1), 7-12.
Zauberman, G., & Lynch Jr, G. (2005). Resource slack and propensity to discount delayed investments of time versus money. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1), 23-37.
Zauberman, G., Kim, K., Malkoc, A., & Bettman, R. (2009). Discounting time and time discounting: Subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(4), 543-556.
Zhao, M., Hoeffler, S., & Zauberman, G. (2007). Mental simulation and preference consistency over time: The role of process-versus outcome-focused thoughts. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(3), 379-388.
Ülkümen, G., & Thomas, M. (2013). Personal relevance and mental simulation amplify the duration framing effect. Journal of Marketing Research, 50(2), 194-206.