ISSN : 1229-8778
Based on the theory of embodied cognition, this study investigated the effect of bodily posture associated with self-regulation on economic decisions involving delayed rewards. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two conditions: Firming condition where participants were asked to clasp a pen firmly against their palm and no-firming condition where participants were asked to hold pen loosely between their index and middle fingers. While maintaining the respective bodily posture, participants were then asked to make a series of monetary decision, which involves choices between smaller, immediate rewards (SIR) and larger, delayed rewards (LDR). The results demonstrated that, compared to those in the no-firming condition, participants in the firming condition were more likely to discount the value of delayed rewards in a lesser degree and choose larger, delayed rewards (LDR) instead of smaller, immediate rewards (SIR). The effect, however, was significant only when the size of LDR was small. The implication of this finding was discussed.
김지숙․김지호 (2013). 손 씻기에 의한 체화된 인지(EC) 소비행동에 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회: 소비자․광고, 14(2), 321-342.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological bulletin, 84(5), 888-918.
Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual review of psychology, 59, 617-645.
Baumeister, R. (2002). Yielding to temptation:Self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4), 670-676.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252-1265.
Baumeister, R. F., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Ego depletion: A resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing. Social Cognition, 18(2), 130-150.
Benzion, U., Rapoport, A., & Yagil, J. (1989). Discount rates inferred from decisions: An experimental study. Management Science, 35, 270-284.
Eskine, K. J., Kacinik, N. A., & Prinz, J. J. (2011). A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22, 295-299.
Förster, J. (2004). How body feedback influences consumers’ evaluation of products. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 416-426.
Frederick, S, Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference:A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351-401.
Green, L., Fristoe, N., & Myerson, J. (1994). Temporal discounting and preference reversals in choice between delayed outcomes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 383-389.
Green, L., Fry, A., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33-36.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1981). Self-control as response strength. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, &C. F. Lowe (Eds.), Quantification of steady-state operant behavior (pp. 3-20). Amsterdam:Elsevier/North Holland Biomedical Press.
Hoch, S. J., & Loewenstein, G. (1991). Time-inconsistent preferences and consumer self-control. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(1), 492-504.
Hung, I. W., & Labroo, A. A. (2011). From firm muscles to firm willpower: Understanding the role of embodied cogntion in self-regulation. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 1046-1064.
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2012). Disgusting smells cause decreased liking of gay men. Emotion, 12(1), 23-27.
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Knobe, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion, 9(3), 435-439.
Kanfer, F. H., & Karoly, P. (1972). Self-control: A behavioristic excursion into the lion's den. Behavioral Therapy, 3, 398-416.
Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future:Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Genereal, 126, 54-70.
Kirby, K. N., & Maraković, N. N. (1995). Modeling myopic decisions: Evidence for hyperbolic delay-disocounting within subjects and amounts. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 64, 22-30.
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 128(1), 78-87.
Labroo, A. A., & Nielsen, J. H. (2010). Half the thrill is in the chase: Twisted inferences from embodied cognitions and brand evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 143-158.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045-1067.
Liljenquist, K., Zhong, C.-B., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). The smell of virtue: clean scents promote reciprocity and charity. Psychological science, 21(3), 381-383.
Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influence on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272-292.
Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior:Vol. 5. The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value (pp. 55-73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., & Bargh, J. A. (2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 705-716.
Metcalfe, J. & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification:Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3-19.
Mischel, W. (1958). Preference for delayed reinforcement: An experimental study of a cultural observation. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 57-61
Mischel, W. (1996). From good intentions to willpower. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 197-218). New York: Guilford Press.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244(4907), 933-938.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1992). Delay of Gratification in Children. In Lowenstein, George; Elster, Jon. (Eds.), Choice Over Time (pp. 147-164). Russell Sage Foundation.
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological bulletin, 126(2), 247-259.
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184-211.
Pizarro, D., Inbar, Y., & Helion, C. (2011). On disgust and moral judgment. Emotion Review, 3(3), 267-268.
Reimann, M., Feye, W., Malter, A. J., Ackerman, J. M., Castaño, R., Garg, N., Kreuzbauer, R., Labroo, A. A., Lee, A. Y., Morrin, M., Nenkov, G. Y., Nielsen, J. H., Perez, M., Pol, G., Rosa, J. A., Yoon, C., & Zhong, C. B. (2012). Embodiment in judgment and choice. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 5(2), 104-123.
Romal, J. B., & Kaplan, B. J. (1995). Difference in self-control among spenders and savers. Psychology-A Quarterly Journal of Human Behavior, 32, 8-17.
Rook, D. W. (1987). The buying impulse. Journal of Consumer Research. 14, 189-199.
Rook, D. W., & Hoch, S. J. (1985). Consuming impulses. In Morris Holbrook and Elizabeth Hirschman (Eds.), Advances in Consumer Research (Vol. 12, pp. 23-27). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.
Rosa, J. A., Garbarino, E. C., & Malter, A. J. (2006). Keeping the body in mind: The influence of body esteem and body boundary aberration on consumer beliefs and purchase intentions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 79-91.
Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. The Review of Economic Studies, 4(2), 155-161.
Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008). With a clean conscience: cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1219-1222.
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1096-1109.
Spellman, B. A., & Schnall, S. (2009). Embodied rationality. Queen’s Law Review, 35, 117-164
Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1660-1672.
Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economic Letters, 8(3), 201-207.
Van den Bergh, B., Dewitte, S., & Warlop, L. (2008). Bikinis instigate generalized impatience in intertemporal choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(2), 85-97.
Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head movements on persuasion:Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219-230.
Zhong, C. -B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451-1452.
Zhong, C. -B., Strejcek, B., & Sivanathan, N. (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 859-862.