바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

logo

The longitudinal study for the relations among communicative perspective-taking, false belief, and executive function

Abstract

The study examined the relations among communicative perspective-taking, false belief, and executive function from the developmental point of view. Fifty-four children participated in the study, whose ages ranged from 46 months to 56 months at the beginning of the study. The children were tested 3 times at intervals of six-months. The eye movements(i. e., the fixation time on the target objects or egocentric objects) were measured for the communicative perspective-taking tasks which were composed of the common-ground and the privileged-ground conditions. Inhibitory control(flag task), working memory(backward-word task), and attention switching(card-sorting task) were also measured for the executive functions. The understanding of false belief was also measured. The results indicated that the egocentric perspectives were predominant at the beginning stage, but significantly decreased as the age level went up. As for the relations among communicative perspective-taking, false belief, and executive function, communicative perspective-taking was not correlated with the false belief understanding, but with working memory. Working memory was the only variable that predicted communicative perspective-taking.

keywords
Submission Date
2017-07-03
Revised Date
2017-08-21
Accepted Date
2017-08-31

Reference

1.

강인선, 이현진 (2015). 운동이 유아의 실행기능에 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회지 발달, 28(3), 209-226.

2.

김영태, 홍경훈, 김경희, 장혜성, 이주연 (2009). 수용 표현 어휘력 검사. 서울: 서울 장애인종합복지관.

3.

이현진 (2011). 마음이론과 실행기능의 발달 및 관계: 한국 아동 자료를 중심으로. 한국심리학회지 발달, 24(4), 99-113.

4.

이현진, 권은영, 전시현 (2016). 의사소통 조망수용: 자기중심적 편향성과 실행기능의역할. 한국심리학회지 발달, 29(4), 81-100.

5.

장유경 (2004). 한국 영아의 초기 어휘발달: 18개월~36개월. 한국심리학회지 발달, 17(4), 91-105.

6.

정미옥, 이현진 (2014). 아동의 의사소통 조망수용, 틀린믿음, 억제통제의 발달. 한국심리학회지 발달, 27(1), 73-94.

7.

Bernard, S., & Deleau, M. (2007). Conversational perspective-taking and false belief attribution:A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 443-460.

8.

Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 893-900.

9.

Brown-Schmidt, S., Gunlogson, C., & Tanenhaus, M. L. (2008). Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation. Cognition, 107, 1122-1134.

10.

Brown-Schmidt, S., & Heller, D. (2013). What language processing can tell us about perspective taking: A reply to Bezuibenhout. Journal of Pragmatics, 60, 279-284.

11.

Deleau, M., & Bernard, S. (2003). Conversational awareness and Theory of mind development. Conference on Developmental Psychology, Milan, 27–31 Aug.

12.

Epley, N., Morewedge, C. K., & Keysar, B. (2004). Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760-768.

13.

Farrar, M. J., Lee, H, Cho, Y-H, Tarmargo, J., & Seung, H. (2013). Language and false belief in Korean-speaking and English-speaking children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 209-221.

14.

Heller, D., Grodner, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference. Cognition, 108, 831-836.

15.

Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233-253.

16.

Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11, 32-38.

17.

Lin, S., Keysar, B., & Epley, N. (2010). Reflexively mindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 551-556.

18.

Maridaki-Kassotaki, K., & Antonopoulou, K. (2011). Examination of the relationship between false-belief understanding and referential communication skills. European Journal of Psychology Education, 26, 75-84.

19.

Miller, M. R., Giesbrecht, G. F., Müller, U., McInerney, R. J., & Kerns, K. A. (2012). A latent variable approach to determining the structure of executive function in preschool children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(3), 395-423.

20.

Nadig, A. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children's on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science, 13(4), 329-336.

21.

Nilsen, E. S., & Fecica, A. (2011). A model communicative perspective-taking for typical and atypical populations of children. Developmental Review, 31, 55-78.

22.

Nilsen, E. S., & Graham, S. A. (2009). The relations between children's communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 220-249.

23.

Papafragou, A. (2002). Mindreading and verbal communication. Mind and Language, 17, 55-67.

24.

Perner, J., Leekams, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-old’s difficulty with false belief: the case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125-129.

25.

Piaget, J. (1926). The language and thought of the child. New York: Harcourt Brace & Company.

26.

Reches, M., & Pérez Pereira, M. (2007). Referential communication abilities and theory of mind development in preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 34, 21-52.

27.

Singer, J. D., & Willet, J. B. (2003). Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis. Oxford, New York.

28.

Wardlow Lane, L. (2013). Individual differences in speakers' perspective taking: The roles of executive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 766-772.

29.

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in yourn children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.

30.

Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science, 18, 600-606.

logo