바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

logo

Informants' characteristics that affect the selective trust of Korean children

Abstract

The present study investigated the characteristics of informants that affect Korean children’s selection of a more reliable source of information with regard to age, gender, and status. Four and five-year-old children were presented with the pictures of two people contrasted in terms of their age, gender, or status. they were then asked who they expected to have more knowledge and which informant they would prefer to ask the name of a novel object. Overall, 5-year-olds selected adults over children, teachers over parents, showing that they consider age and status in their selective trust. By comparison, 4-year-olds were not affected by these characteristics in choosing a knowledgeable informant. Interestingly, while boys selected teachers as a reliable source over parents, girls’s selective trust was not affected by the status of the informant, suggesting that status is a characteristic that boys tend to consider at an earlier age. These results suggest that the informant’s characteristics may affect the development of selective trust differently according to the child’s age and gender, and that Korean children’s development of selective trust may not follow the same developmental pattern as children studied in the US or Europe.

keywords
Submission Date
2018-03-03
Revised Date
2018-03-30
Accepted Date
2018-04-02

Reference

1.

고연정, 최영은 (2011). 만 3-4세 아동의 신뢰성 판단에 관찰 경험이 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 24(4), 151-162.

2.

고연정, 최영은 (2013). 만 3세 아동의 타인 증언신뢰 편향성 검증. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 26(1), 197-214.

3.

정소미, 최영은 (2017). 한국 아동의 신뢰성 판단에 정보제공자의 연령이 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 30(3), 67-82

4.

최영은 (2016). 의심의 기술 발달: 선택적 신뢰와 증거성 추론. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 29(1), 63-82.

5.

Bascandziev, I., & Harris, P. L. (2014). In beauty we trust: Children prefer information from more attractive informants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 94-99.

6.

Bascandziev, I., & Harris, P. L. (2016). The beautiful and the accurate: Are children’s selective trust decisions biased?. Journal of experimental child psychology, 152, 92-105.

7.

Berk, L. E. (2013). Child Development. 8th Edition. Pearson International Edition.

8.

Birch, S. A., Vauthier, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2008). Three-and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition, 107(3), 1018-1034.

9.

Bond, R., & Smith, P. B. (1996). Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch's(1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychological bulletin, 119(1), 111.

10.

Chan, C. C., & Tardif, T. (2013). Knowing better:the role of prior knowledge and culture in trust in testimony. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 591-601.

11.

Corriveau, K., & Harris, P. L. (2009a). Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Developmental science, 12(3), 426-437.

12.

Corriveau, K., & Harris, P. L. (2009b). Preschoolers continue to trust a more accurate informant 1 week after exposure to accuracy information. Developmental science, 12(1), 188-193.

13.

Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2010). Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Developmental psychology, 46(2), 437.

14.

Corriveau, K. H., Kinzler, K. D., & Harris, P. L. (2013). Accuracy trumps accent in children's endorsement of object labels. Developmental psychology, 49(3), 470.

15.

Elashi, F. B., & Mills, C. M. (2014). Do children trust based on group membership or prior accuracy? The role of novel group membership in children’s trust decisions. Journal of experimental child psychology, 128, 88-104.

16.

Jaswal, V. K., Croft, A. C., Setia, A. R., & Cole, C. A. (2010). Young children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1541-1547.

17.

Jaswal, V. K., & Neely, L. A. (2006). Adults don't always know best preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17(9), 757-758.

18.

Kinzler, K. D., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental science, 14(1), 106-111.

19.

Koenig, M. A., Clement, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15(10), 694-698.

20.

Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child development, 76(6), 1261-1277.

21.

Koenig, M. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2010). Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: possible mechanisms. Developmental psychology, 46(4), 815.

22.

Ma, L., & Woolley, J. D. (2013). Young children's sensitivity to speaker gender when learning from others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(1), 100-119.

23.

McDonald, K. P., & Ma, L. (2015). Dress Nicer=Know More? Young Children’s Knowledge Attribution and Selective Learning Based on How Others Dress. PloS one, 10(12), e0144424.

24.

Rogoff, B. (1993). Children’s guided participation and participatory ap- propriation in sociocultural activity. In R. Wozniak & K. Fischer (Eds.), Development in context: Acting and thinking in specific environments (pp. 121-153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

25.

Rosenbaum, E. (2012). TICKLE parts [Web Post image] Retrieved January 17, 2018 from https://www.flickr.com/photos/ericrosenbaum/7927153216

26.

Shutts, K., Banaji, M. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Social categories guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Developmental science, 13(4), 599-610.

27.

Taylor, M. G. (2013). Gender influences on children’s selective trust of adult testimony. Journal of experimental child psychology, 115(4), 672-690.

28.

Taylor, M., Cartwright, B. S., & Bowden, T. (1991). Perspective taking and theory of mind: Do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers' knowledge?. Child development, 62(6), 1334-1351.

logo