ISSN : 1229-0718
One pervasive facet of human interactions is the tendency to favor ingroups over outgroups. What are the developmental origins of ingroup favoritism? Among several accounts proposed to answer this question, an evolutionary perspective holds that ingroup favoritism reflects in part an abstract and early-emerging sociomoral expectation of ingroup support. This paper reviews the key findings supporting this view and suggests future directions.
김은영, & 송현주 (2016). 만 5-6 세 한국 아동의 사회비교에 영향을 주는 양육자 요인. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 29(1), 101-116.
김혜리, 정명숙, 손정우, 박 민, 엄진섭, 문은옥, 천영운, 최현숙, 이수미. (2013). 아동의 인지적․정서적 공감 능력과 사회적 행동간의 관계. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 26(1), 255-275.
이미영, 장은진 (2015). 학교폭력 가해자가 경험한 학교폭력 맥락에 관한 질적 연구. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 28(3), 115-140.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., He, Z., Sloane, S., Setoh, P., Jin, K., Wu, D., & Bian, L. (2015). Psychological and sociomoral reasoning in infancy. In M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver (Eds), E. Borgida, & J. A. Bargh (Assoc. Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology: Vol.1. Attitudes and social cognition (pp. 79-150). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Balliet, D., Wu, J., & De Dreu, C. K. (2014). Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1556-1581.
Barragan, R. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2014). Rethinking natural altruism: Simple reciprocal interactions trigger children’s benevolence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 17071-17074.
Bian, L., Sloane, S., & Baillargeon, R. (2018). Infants expect ingroup support to override fairness when resources are limited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 2705-2710.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books
Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitivemotivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307-324.
Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love and outgroup hate?. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 429-444.
Carpenter, M., Uebel, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old infants. Child Development, 84, 1511-1518.
Cikara, M., Bruneau, E. G., & Saxe, R. R. (2011). Us and them: Intergroup failures of empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 149-153.
Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science, 17, 1003-1011.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary psychology: New perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 201-229.
Dovidio, J. F., Johnson, J. D., Gaertner, S. L., Pearson, A. R., Saguy, T., & Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2010). Empathy and intergroup relations. In M. Mikulincer, & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Prosocial motives, emotions, and behavior: The better angels of our nature. (pp. 393-408). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793-811.
Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information across the life span: Theory and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 19-46.
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454, 1079-1083.
Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14, 1012-1020.
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55-130). Academic Press.
Gramzow, R. H., & Gaertner, L. (2005). Self-esteem and favoritism toward novel in-groups: the self as an evaluative base. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 801-815.
Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants:the role of domain-general versus domainspecific processes in the development of social evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 614.
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557-559.
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 19931-19936.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (2001). On a folk theory of society: Children, evolution, and mental representations of social groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 107-117.
Hogg, M. A. (2003). Social identity. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The handbook of self and identity (pp. 462-479). New York:Guilford.
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., & Chen, F. S. (2007). Evidence for infants' internal working models of attachment. Psychological Science, 18, 501-502.
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., Chen, F. S., Stern, H. L., Ok, S. J., & Barth, M. (2010). At the intersection of social and cognitive development: Internal working models of attachment in infancy. Cognitive Science, 34, 807-825.
Jin, K., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Infants possess an abstract expectation of ingroup support. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 8199-8204.
Jin, K., Houston, J. L., Baillargeon, R., Groh, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2018). Young infants expect an unfamiliar adult to comfort a crying baby: Evidence from a standard violation-ofexpectation task and a novel infant-triggeredvideo task. Cognitive Psychology, 102, 1-20.
Killen, M., & Cooley, S. (2014). Morality, exclusion, & prejudice. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 340-360). New York, NY:Psychology Press.
Killen, M., Rutland, A., Abrams, D., Mulvey, K. L., & Hitti, A. (2013). Development of intra ‐and intergroup judgments in the context of moral and social‐conventional norms. Child Development, 84, 1063-1080.
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 12577-12580.
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). ‘Native’objects and collaborators:Infants' object choices and acts of giving reflect favor for native over foreign speakers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 67-81.
Kotovsky, L., & Baillargeon, R. (1994). Calibration-based reasoning about collision events in 11-month-old infants. Cognition, 51, 107-129.
Kotovsky, L., & Baillargeon, R. (1998). The development of calibration-based reasoning about collision events in young infants. Cognition, 67, 311-351.
Levine, M., Prosser, A., Evans, D., & Reicher, S. (2005). Identity and emergency intervention:How social group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 443-453.
Molenberghs, P. (2013). The neuroscience of in-group bias. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 1530-1536.
Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition, 108, 222-231.
Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children's attention to environmental messages about groups: Social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Development, 77, 847-860.
Plötner, M., Over, H., Carpenter, M., &Tomasello, M. (2015). The effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on children’s prosocial behavior, liking, affiliation, and trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 161-173.
Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118, 57-75.
Renno, M. P., & Shutts, K. (2015). Children’s social category-based giving and its correlates:Expectations and preferences. Developmental Psychology, 51, 533-543.
Rhodes, M. (2012). Naïve theories of social groups. Child Development, 83, 1900-1916.
Rhodes, M., & Chalik, L. (2013). Social categories as markers of intrinsic interpersonal obligations. Psychological Science, 24, 999-1006.
Richter, N., Over, H., & Dunham, Y. (2016). The effects of minimal group membership on young preschoolers' social preferences, estimates of similarity, and behavioral attribution. Collabra, 2, 1-8.
Schmidt, M. F., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PloS one, 6, e23223.
Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., McKee, C. B., &Spelke, E. S. (2009). Social information guides infants' selection of foods. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 1-17.
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., &Park, L. (1997). The “big three” of morality (autonomy, community, and divinity), and the “big three” explanations of suffering. In A. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119-169). New York: Routledge.
Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological Science, 23, 196-204.
Spokes, A. C., & Spelke, E. S. (2017). The cradle of social knowledge: infants’ reasoning about caregiving and affiliation. Cognition, 159, 102-116
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., &Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149-178.
Tafjel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). Social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301-1303.
Yamagishi, T., & Mifune, N. (2009). Social exchange and solidarity: In-group love or out-group hate?. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 229-237.