바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

logo

Preschoolers' ability to use gaze information when understanding others' preferences

Abstract

Previous research has shown that 5- to 6-year-olds, perhaps not younger children, can use the duration of eye gaze to understand others’ preferences(Einav & Hood, 2006; Montgomery, Bach, & Moran, 1998). However, the experimental situation used in the previous research might not have been ambiguous in terms of the ultimate goals of the actor's actions. Thus children might have had difficulty linking an actor's gazing behaviors and future action preferences. Therefore, we made a change in our experiment and aimed to further examine the nature of Korean preschoolers’ ability to use eye gaze duration when understanding others’ preferences. Four-year-old Korean children participated in study 1 and 3.5-year-olds in study 2. Both of the experiments demonstrated that 3.5-year-old and older Korean children can consider other people’s looking behavior, especially eye-gaze duration, as a cue to others’ preferences. Possible cultural and contextual factors are discussed to account for the discrepancy between our and previous findings.

keywords
Submission Date
2007-10-15
Revised Date
2007-11-13
Accepted Date
2007-11-14

Reference

1.

김혜리 (1997). 아동의 마음에 대한 이해 발달: 틀린믿음에 대한 이해로 살펴 본 마음-이론의 발달. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 10(1), 74-91.

2.

Avis, J., & Harris, P. L. (1991). Belief- desire reasoning among Baka children: Evidence for a universal conception of mind. Child Development, 62, 460-467.

3.

Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff- Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significant of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379-398.

4.

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind"? Cognition, 21, 37-46.

5.

Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 1352-1366.

6.

Dunn, J., & Hughes, C. (1998). Young children's understanding of emotions within close friendship. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 171-190.

7.

Einav, S., & Hood, B. L. (2006). Children's use of the temporal dimension of gaze for inferring preference. Developmental Psychology, 42, 142-152.

8.

Flavell, J. H. (2004). Development of knowledge about vision. In D. T. Levin (Ed), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adult and children(pp. 13-36). Cambridge, MA, US: MIT Press.

9.

Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1995). The development of children's knowledge about attentional focus. Developmental Psychology, 31, 706-712.

10.

Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Kiraly, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.

11.

Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165-193.

12.

Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. American Psychological Society, 14, 402-408.

13.

Lewis, C., Freeman, N. H., Kyriaskidou, C., Maridaki-Kassotaki, K., & Berridge, D. M. (1996). Social influences on false- belief access: Sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67, 2930-2947.

14.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation, Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.

15.

Montgomery, D. E., Bach, L. M., & Moran, C. (1998). Children's use of looking behavior as a cue to detect another's goal. Child Development, 69, 692-705.

16.

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291-310.

17.

Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228-1238.

18.

Phillips, A. T., Wellman, H. M., & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85, 53-78.

19.

Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14-to 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12-21.

20.

Sodian, B., & Thoemer, C. (2004). Infants' understanding of looking, pointing, and reaching as a cues to goal directed action. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 289-316.

21.

Sue, S. K., Chiu, C. Y., Hong, Y. Y., Leung, K., Peng, K., & Morris, M. W. (1999). Self organization and social organization: American and Chinese constructions. In T. R. Tyler, R. Kramer, & O. John (Eds.), The psychology of the social self(pp. 193-222). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

22.

Triandis, H. C. (1989). Cross-culture studies of individualism and collectivism. Nebraska Symposium of Motivation, 37, 41-133.

23.

Vinden, P. G. (1999). Children's understanding of mind and emotion: A multi-culture study. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 19-48.

24.

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.

25.

Woodward, A. L. (2003). Infant’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science, 6, 297-311.

26.

Youngblade, L. M., & Dunn, J. (1995). Individual difference in young children's pretend play with mother and sibling. Child Development, 66, 1472-1492.

logo