바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

Korean Journal of Psychology: General

  • KOREAN
  • P-ISSN1229-067X
  • E-ISSN2734-1127
  • KCI

The Neural Bases of Theory of Mind

Korean Journal of Psychology: General / Korean Journal of Psychology: General, (P)1229-067X; (E)2734-1127
2007, v.26 no.2, pp.39-62




Abstract

Theory of mind (ToM) refers to the knowledge about the mind that we have mental states―such as intention, desire and belief―and our mental states cause our action is recognized as theory of mind. A person with theory of mind comes to read people’s actions in terms of their mental states, that is, mindreading. In this review, we summarize recent work that has illuminated the neural bases of mindreading. Lesion studies has suggested that damage to brain areas such as right hemisphere, orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala were correlated with ToM deficits. Neuroimaging studies of normal adults have consistently demonstrated the importance of particular brain regions for ToM, the superior temporal sulcus (STS), temporal pole (TP) and the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). Findings from brain imaging and lesion studies indicate that ToM ability is supported by a widely distributed neural system. The theoretical importance of ToM for everyday life requires further studies in more various populations.

keywords
마음이론, 뇌 영상 연구, 내측 전전두피질, 상측두구, 측두극, Theory of Mind, brain imaging study, medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, temporal pole

Reference

1.

박민, 이승복, 윤효운, 김소영, 김혜리 (2006). 지향성 탐지기: 과제에 따른 뇌 활성화 영역 차이. 한국심리학회지: 실험, 18(2), 127-138.

2.

박민, 이승복, 윤효운, 정우림, 김혜리 (2007). 지향성 탐지 기제에서의 개인차: 전공에 따른 뇌 활성화 영역. 인지과학, 18(2), 49-67.

3.

이승복, 박민, 윤효운, 김혜리 (2006). 지향성 탐지 과정의 뇌 활성화: 기능적 자기공명 영상 연구. 인지과학, 17(1), 1-13.

4.

Abu-Akel, A. (2003). A neurobiological mapping of theory of mind. Brain Research Reviews, 43(1), 29-40.

5.

Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4), 268-277.

6.

Astington, J. W., Pelletier, J., & Homer, B. (2002). Theory of mind and epistemological development: The relation between children’s second-order false-belief understanding and their ability to reason about evidence. New Ideas in Psychology, 20(2), 131-144.

7.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). 마음맹: 자폐증과 마음이론에 관한 과학에세이[Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind]. (김혜리, 이현진 역). 서울: 시그마프레스. (원전은 1995년에 출판)

8.

Baron-Cohen, S., & Sally, W. (2004). The Empathy Quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163-175.

9.

Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 407-418.

10.

Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Moriarty, J., Schmitz, B., Costa, D., & Ell, P. (1994). Recognition of mental state terms: Clinical findings in children with autism and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 165(5), 640-649.

11.

Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., & Williams, S, (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11(6), 1891-1898.

12.

Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.). (2000). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

13.

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241-251.

14.

Blakemore, S. J., Boyer, P., Pachot-Clouard, M., Meltzoff, A., Segebarth, C., & Decety, J. (2003). The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 13(8), 837-844.

15.

Blakemore, S., Winston, J., & Frith, U. (2004). Social cognitive neuroscience: Where are we heading? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(5), 216-222.

16.

Brüne, M., & Brüne-Cohrs, U. (2006). Theory of mind―Evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(4), 437-455.

17.

Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M. C., & Decety, J. (2000). A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 11(2), 157-166.

18.

Calarge, C., Andreasen, N. C., O’Leary, D. S. (2003). Visualizing how one brain understands another: A PET study of theory of mind. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 1954-1964.

19.

Calder, A. J., Lawrence, D., Keane, J., Scott, S. K., Owen, A. I., Christoffels, I., & Young, A. W. (2002). Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), 1129-1138.

20.

Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125(8), 1839-1849.

21.

Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. NeuroImage 12(3), 314-325.

22.

Chakrabarti, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Empathizing: Neurocognitive developmental mechanisms and individual differences. In S. Anders, G. Ende, M. Junghofer, J. Kissler & Wildgruber, D (Eds.), Progress in brain research: Vol. 156. Understanding emotions (pp. 403-418). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

23.

Channon, S., & Crawford, S. (2000). The effects of anterior lesions on performance on a story comprehension test: Left anterior impairment on a theory of mind-type task. Neuropsychologia, 38(7), 1006-1017.

24.

Cuerva, A. G., Sabe, L., Kuzis, G., Tiberti, C., Dorrego, F., Starkstein, S. E. (2001). Theory of mind and pragmatic abilities in dementia. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 14(3), 153-158.

25.

den Ouden, H., Frith, U., Frith, C., Blakemore, S. J. (2005). Thinking about intentions. NeuroImage, 28(4), 787-796.

26.

Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581-604.

27.

Ferstl, E. C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). What does the frontomedian cortex contribute to language processing: Coherence or theory of mind. NeuroImage, 17(3), 1599-1612.

28.

Fine, C., Lumsden, J., & Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Dissociation between ‘theory of mind’ and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain, 124(2), 287-298.

29.

Fletcher, P. C., Happé, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57(2), 109-128.

30.

Focquaert, F., Steven, M.S., Wolford, G.L., Colden, A., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing cognitive traits in the sciences and humanities. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(3), 619-625.

31.

Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459-473.

32.

Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 77-83.

33.

Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An fMRI study of “theory of mind” in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 11-21.

34.

Gregory, C., Lough, S., Stone, V., Erzinclioglu, S., Martin, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: Theoretical and practical implications. Brain, 125(4), 752-764.

35.

Griffin, R., Friedman, O., Ween, J., Winner, E., Happé, F., & Brownell, H (2006). Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality, 11(3), 195-225.

36.

Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129-154.

37.

Happé, F., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (1999). Acquired ‘theory of mind’ impairments following stroke. Cognition, 70(3), 211-240.

38.

Heider, F., & Simmel, M., (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243-259.

39.

Hooker, C. I., Paller, K. A., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., Mesulam, M.-M., & Reber, P. J. (2003). Brain networks for analyzing eye gaze. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(2), 406-418.

40.

Kampe, K. K., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12), 5258-5263.

41.

Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H., & Temple, E. (2006). Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’: An fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain and Language, 98(2), 210-220.

42.

Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H., & Temple, E. (2007a). Children’s and adults’ neural bases of verbal and nonverbal ‘theory of mind’. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1522-1532.

43.

Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H., & Temple, E. (2007b). Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’ in American and Japanese children. Brain Research, 1164, 95-107.

44.

Leekam, S. (1993). Children’s understanding of mind. In M. Bennett (Ed.), The development of social cognition: The child as psychologist (pp. 26-61). New York: The Guilford Press.

45.

Lough, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Measuring and modifying abnormal social cognition in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53(2), 639-646.

46.

Lough, S., Gregory, C., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). Dissociation of social cognition and executive function in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase, 7(2), 123-130.

47.

Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Mori, T., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. (2007). Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61(4), 355-363.

48.

Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 337-344.

49.

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a ‘theory of mind’? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526.

50.

Rowe, A. D., Bullock, P. R., Polkey, C. E., & Morris, R. G. (2001). ‘Theory of mind’ impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. Brain, 124(3), 600-616.

51.

Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2003). What do you believe versus what do you think they believe: A neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 2475-2480.

52.

Saltzman, J., Strauss, E., Hunter, M., & Archibald, S. (2000). Theory of mind and executive functions in normal human aging and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(7), 781-788.

53.

Saxe, R. (2006). Why and how to study theory of mind with fMRI. Brain Research, 1079(1), 57-65.

54.

Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1835-1842.

55.

Saxe, R., & Wexler, A. (2005). Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia, 43(10), 1391–1399.

56.

Saxe, R., Carey, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 87–124.

57.

Saxe, R., Xiao, D. K., Kovacs, G., Perrett, D. I., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia, 42(11), 1435–1446.

58.

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer R., Berger, B. D., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2003). Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain damage: The role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 324-337.

59.

Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2002). Neural systems involved in ‘theory of mind’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(6), 463-471.

60.

Siegal, M., Carrington, J., & Radel, M. (1996). Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following: Right hemisphere damage. Brain and Language, 53(1), 40-50.

61.

Singer, T. (2006). The neural basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(6), 855-863.

62.

Snowden, J. S., Gibbons, Z. C., Blackshaw, A., Doubleday, E., Thompson, J., Craufurd, D. Foster, J., Happé, F., & Neary, D. (2003). Social cognition in frontotemporal dementia and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 41(6), 688-701.

63.

Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 640-656.

64.

Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J., & Young, A. W. (2003). Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 209-220.

65.

Stuss, D. T., Gallup, G. G., & Alexander, M. P. (2001). The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain, 124(2), 279-286.

66.

Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2001). Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 78(2), 224-232.

67.

Tompkins, C. A., Scharp, V. L., Fassbinder, W., Meigh, K., & Armstrong, E. M. (2006). ‘Theory of mind’ in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Brain and Language, 99(1-2), 57-58.

68.

Varley, R., & Siegal, M. (2000). Evidence for cognition without grammar from causal reasoning and ‘theory of mind’ in an agrammatic aphasic patient. Current Biology, 10(12), 723-726.

69.

Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, S. P., Newen, A., Herrmann, S., Happé, F., Falkai, P., Maier, W., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., & Zilles, K. (2001). Mind reading: Neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage, 14(1), 170-181.

70.

Völlm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., Deakin, J. F. W., & Elliott, R. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29(1), 90-98.

71.

Wicker, B., Ruby, P., Royet, J. P., & Fonlupt, P. (2003). A relation between rest and the self in the brain? Brain Research Reviews, 43(2), 224-230.

72.

Winner, E., Brownell, H., Happé, F., Blum, A., & Pincus, D. (1998). Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 62(1), 89-106.

Korean Journal of Psychology: General