바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

Korean Journal of Psychology: General

The development of abilities to infer others' dispositions in preschoolers

Korean Journal of Psychology: General / Korean Journal of Psychology: General, (P)1229-067X; (E)2734-1127
2007, v.26 no.2, pp.83-100


Abstract

The present research examined whether preschoolers can infer others’ dispositions in explicit reasoning tasks. Children watched two types of events in which a circle, a triangle and a square interacted. In one, either the triangle or square helped the circle climb a hill. In the other, the other agent hindered the circle from climbing the hill. After watching the events, children were asked (1) to predict which agent the circle would approach in a novel context and (2) to infer which agent was nice or bad. The children had to provide either verbal or gestural responses to the questions. The results showed that 2.5-year-olds but not younger children could predict the circle’s preference-based action. When inferring the agents’ traits, only 3.5-year-olds succeeded in the task; They did so even when the helping agent’s action did not lead to the achievement of the circle’s goal. The results are discussed in terms of the distinction between the implicit and explicit levels of knowledge of others’ dispositions.

keywords
dispositions, traits, theory of mind, explicit knowledge, 성향 이해, 특질 이해, 마음 이론, 외현적 지식

Reference

1.

구재선, 김혜리, 김경미, 양혜영, 고숙남 (2006). 아동의 성격 특질에 대한 이해 발달: 특질 추론과 상황적, 시간적 안정성을 중심으로. 한국심리학회지: 발달, 19, 1-20.

2.

김혜리 (1997). 아동의 마음에 대한 이해 발달: 틀린믿음에 대한 이해로 살펴본 마음-이론의 발달, 한국심리학회지: 발달, 10, 74-91.

3.

서상규 (1998). 말뭉치 분석에 기반을 둔 낱말 빈도의 조사와 그 응용-'연세 말뭉치'를 중심으로. 한글, 242, 225-270.

4.

Alibali, M. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993). Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: what the hands reveal about a child’s state of mind. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 468-523.

5.

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985) Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37-46.E

6.

Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Unwilling versus unable: chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action Developmental Science, 7, 488-498.

7.

Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315-330.

8.

Church, R. B. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23, 43-71.

9.

Clement, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377-397.

10.

Clements, W. A., Rustin, C. L., & McCallum, S. (2000). Promoting the transition from implicit to explicit understanding: A training study of false belief. Developmental Science, 3, 81–92.

11.

Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237-267.

12.

Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 735-808.

13.

Fodor, J. (1992). A theory of the child’s theory of mind. Cognition, 44, 283-296.

14.

Garnham, W. A., & Ruffman, T. (2001). Doesn’t see, doesn’t know: Is anticipatory looking really related to understanding of belief? Developmental Science, 4, 94-100.

15.

Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165-193.

16.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). From meta-processes to conscious access: evidence from children’s metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition, 23, 95-147.

17.

Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychological Science, 14, 402-408.

18.

Kuhlmeier, V., & Wynn, K. (2003). 9-month-olds’ understanding of intentionality. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting in Tampa, Florida.

19.

Leslie, A. M. (1994a). Pretending and believing: Issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition, 50, 211-238.

20.

Leslie, A. M. (1994b). ToMM, ToBy, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119-148). New York: Cambridge University Press.

21.

Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16, 601-608.

22.

Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838-850.

23.

Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Nature, 308, 255-258.

24.

Perner, J., & Clements, W. A. (1999). From an implicit to an explicit theory of mind. In Y. Rossetti & A. Revonsuo (Eds.) Beyond dissociation: Interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing (pp. 273-293), Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

25.

Phillips, A. T., Wellman, H. M., & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85, 53-78.

26.

Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of child. London: Kegan Paul.

27.

Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1995). Origins of human social competence. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 205-218). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

28.

Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1997). Infants attribute value +/− to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 848-856.

29.

Reid, V. M., Csibra, G., Belsky, J., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). Neural correlates of the perception of goal-directed action in infants. Acta Psychologica, 124, 129-138.

30.

Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infants’ use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person’s emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017-1025.

31.

Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12-21.

32.

Schlottman, A., & Surian, L. (1999). Do 9-month-olds perceive causation-at-a-distance? Perception, 28, 1105-1113.

33.

Shimizu, Y. A., & Johnson, S. C. (2004). Infants’ attribution of a goal to a morphologically unfamiliar agent. Developmental Science, 7, 425-430.

34.

Song, H., Baillargeon, R., & Fisher, C. (2005). Can infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action? Cognition, 98, B45-B55.

35.

Song, H., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Can 9.5-month-old infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action on objects? Acta Psychologica, 124, 79-105.

36.

Thoermer, C., & Sodian, B. (2001). Preverbal infants’ understanding of referential gestures. First Language, 21, 245-264.

37.

Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639-650.

38.

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.

39.

Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1-34.

40.

Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors, Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145-160.

41.

Woodward, A. L., & Guajardo, J. J. (2002). Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action. Cognitive Development, 17, 1061-1084.

42.

Woodward, A. L., & Sommerville, J. A. (2000). Twelve-month-old infants interpret action in context. Psychological Science, 11, 73-77.

Korean Journal of Psychology: General