바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

Korean Journal of Psychology: General

  • KOREAN
  • P-ISSN1229-067X
  • E-ISSN2734-1127
  • KCI

Korean adults' beliefs about the essence of children's traits

Korean Journal of Psychology: General / Korean Journal of Psychology: General, (P)1229-067X; (E)2734-1127
2008, v.27 no.2, pp.403-423


Abstract

This study examined four questions. First, what kind of view do Korean adults have about the children's traits? Second, do adults' beliefs of children's traits have conceptual coherence? Third, can adults' beliefs about children's trait vary depending on the types of traits? Last, do adults' beliefs about positive and negative traits of children differ from each other? One hundred-two Korean mothers participated in Study 1, and one hundred-two Korean college students participated in Study 2. The results indicated that there was no significant differences between Korean mothers and college students in their views about the essence of children's traits. Overall, the results showed that Korean adults have incremental views about children's traits: they believe the children's traits are changeable and affected by the environment. This research is an initial attempt to investigate the nature of Korean adults' beliefs about children's traits. The results will provide a foundation for future studies on the development of beliefs about traits, and the origins of Korean parenting styles.

keywords
특질, 본질, 사회 인지, 사람 지각, traits, essence, social cognition, person perception

Reference

1.

Berndt, T. J., Cheung, P. C., Lau, S., Hau, K.-T., & Lew, W. J. F. (1993). Perceptions of parenting in mainland China, Taiwan, and Hong Kong: Sex differences and societal differences. Developmental Psychology, 29, 156-164.

2.

Cain, K. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1995). The relation between motivational patterns and achievement cognitions through the elementary school years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 25-52.

3.

Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 47-63.

4.

Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. –P., & Yzerbyt, V. (2006). Lay theories of essentialism. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 9, 25-42.

5.

Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267-285.

6.

Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories: Individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 644-656.

7.

Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.

8.

Erdley, C., Cain, K., Loomis, C., Dumas-Hines, F. & Dweck, C. S. (1997). The relations among children's social goals, implicit personality theories and responses to social failure. Developmental Psychology, 33, 263-272.

9.

Gelman, S. A. (2003). The Essential Child. Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

10.

Gelman, S. A., Heyman, G. D., & Legare, C. H. (2007) Developmental changes in the coherence of essentialist beliefs about psychological characteristics. Child Development, 78, 757-774.

11.

Gottfried, G. M., Gelman, S. A., & Schultz, J. (1999). Children's understanding of the brain: From early essentialism to biological theory, Cognitive Development, 14(1), 147-174.

12.

Haslam, N., Bastian, B., & Bissett, M. (2004). Essentialist beliefs about personality and their implications. Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 30, 1661-1673.

13.

Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(1), 113-127.

14.

Heine, S. J. (2001). Self as cultural product: An examination of East Asian and North American selves. Journal of Personality, 69, 881-906.

15.

Heine, S. J. (2005). Where is the evidence for pancultural self-enhancement?: A reply to Sedikides, Gaertner, and Toguchi (2003). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 531–538.

16.

Heine S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D., Tkata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C., et al. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599-615.

17.

Henderson, V., & Dweck, C. S. (1990). Motivation and achievement. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp.308-329). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

18.

Heyman, G. D., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Children’s thinking about traits: Implications for judgments of the self and others. Child Development, 64, 391-403.

19.

Heyman, G. D. & Gelman, S. A., (1999). The use of trait labels in making psychological inferences. Child Development 70, 604–619.

20.

Holloway, S. D. (1988). Concepts of ability and effort in Japan and the United States. Review of Educational Research, 58, 327-345.

21.

Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., & Larson, S. L. (1998). Do parent practices to encourage academic competence influence the social adjustment of young European American and Chinese American children? Developmental Psychology, 34, 747-756.

22.

Keil, F.C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development, Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press.

23.

Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B., & Story, T. (2002). Young children’s beliefs about the stability of traits: Protective optimism. Child Development, 73, 1408-1430.

24.

Lockhart K. L., Nakashima, N., Inagaki, K., and Keil, F. C. (2008). From ugly ducking to swan? Japanese and American beliefs about the stability and origins of traits. Cognitive Development, 23, 155-179.

25.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review. 98(2), 224-253.

26.

Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural Practices Emphasize Influence in the United States and Adjustment in Japan, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 311-323.

27.

Norenzayan, A., Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural similarities and differences in social inference: evidence from behavioral predictions and lay theories of behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 109-120.

28.

Rothbart, M., & Taylor, M. (1992). Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds? In G. Semin & K. Fiedler (Eds.), Language and social cognition (pp. 11-36). London: Sage.

29.

Ji, L-J., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E., (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of personality and social psychology, 78(5), 943-955.

30.

Su, S. K., Chiu, C.-Y., Hong, Y.-Y., Leung, K., Peng, K., & Morris, M. W. (1999). Self organization and social organization: American and Chinese constructions. In T. R. Tyler, R. Kramer, & O. John (Eds.), The psychology of the social self (pp. 193-222). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

31.

Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review. 96(3), 506-520.

32.

Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. (1984). Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in America and Japan. American Psychologist, 39, 955-969.

Korean Journal of Psychology: General