바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

logo

선행차량 추월에 대한 위험판단에서의 성별차이: 민감도, 반응편파 및 반응시간을 중심으로

Gender Difference in Risk Perception for Overtaking: Focused on Sensitivity, Bias and Reaction Time

한국심리학회지: 일반 / Korean Journal of Psychology: General, (P)1229-067X; (E)2734-1127
2016, v.35 no.1, pp.121-141
https://doi.org/10.22257/kjp.2016.03.35.1.121
박동진 (부산대학교 심리학과)
이재식 (부산대학교)
  • 다운로드 수
  • 조회수

초록

본 연구에서는 선행차량 추월 상황에 대한 실험참가자들의 위험지각 양상을 성별에 따라 비교하였다. 추월상황은 도로유형과 운전속도 조건을 달리하여 동영상 형태로 제시하였고, 각 조건에서 관찰되는 실험참가자들의 반응민감도, 반응편파 및 반응시간을 종속측정치로 분석하였다. 본 연구의 주요 결과와 시사점을 요약하면 다음과 같다. 첫째, 추월판단에서의 민감도는 여성에 비해 남성이 더 높았고, 반응편파는 남성보다 여성의 경우 더 보수적으로 반응하는 경향이 관찰되었는데 이러한 차이는 운전속도가 느린 조건과 굴곡이 있는 도로에서 각각 더 두드러졌다. 둘째, 여성은 남성에 비해 직선도로보다는 굴곡이 있는 도로조건에서, 그리고 운전속도가 빠른 조건보다는 느린 조건에서 더 지연된 판단 반응시간을 보였다. 셋째, 여성은 남성에 비해 탈루와 정기각 반응을 더 느리게 결정하는 경향을 보였다. 본 연구의 결과들은 추월판단 과제에서의 성별 차이를 이해하기 위해서는 도로유형이나 운전속도와 같은 물리적 요인뿐만 아니라 운전자들의 판단정확도와 반응편파, 그리고 판단시간 등을 통합적으로 고려해야 한다는 것을 시사한다.

keywords
추월 위험판단, 성별차이, 반응민감도, 반응편파, 반응시간, overtaking-risk judgment, gender difference, response sensitivity, response bias, reaction time

Abstract

Gender difference in risk perception for overtaking was explored with respect to sensitivity, response bias, and reaction time. The results showed the followings. First, although male drivers showed higher response sensitivity and more lenient response bias than female drivers in overtaking judgment in general. this difference was prominent in straight lane and slow driving speed conditions than in curved lane or fast driving speed condition. Second, female drivers tended to show slower overtaking judgment than male drivers, especially in the conditions of straight lane and slow driving speed. Third, female drivers' reaction time in miss and correct rejection was slower than male drivers. These results suggested the necessity of considering physical condition of overtaking situation as well as various dependent measures such as response sensitivity, response bias and reaction time in understand gender difference in overtaking judgment.

keywords
추월 위험판단, 성별차이, 반응민감도, 반응편파, 반응시간, overtaking-risk judgment, gender difference, response sensitivity, response bias, reaction time

참고문헌

1.

국토해양부 (2012). 도로설계규정. 세종특별시

2.

박제진, 하태준 (2000). 지방부 도로에서의 최소필요 추월시거 산정기준에 관한 연구.대한교통학회 학술대회지, 38, 245-250.

3.

이순철 (2000). 교통행동의 심리학적 이해. 한국심리학회지: 문화 및 사회문제, 6, 119-143.

4.

이재식 (2012). 과속운전과 교통사고에 미치는운전분노와 성격 5요인의 상호작용.Journal of the Korean Data Analysis Society, 14,2081-2095.

5.

Berndt, H., Emmert, J., & Dietmayer, K. (2008). Continuous driver intention recognition with hidden markov models. In Intelligent Transportation Systems, 2008. ITSC 2008. 11th International IEEE Conference on (pp. 1189-1194). IEEE.

6.

Brown, I. D. (1994). Driver fatigue. Human Factors, 36, 298-314.

7.

Clarke, D. D., Ward, P. J., & Jones, J. (1998). Overtaking road-accidents: Differences in manoeuvre as a function of driver age. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 30, 455-467.

8.

Clarke, D. D., Ward, P. J., & Jones, J. (1998). Overtaking road accidents: Differences in manoeuvre as a function of driver age. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 30, 455-467.

9.

Clarke, D. D., Ward, P., Bartle, C., & Truman, W. (2006). Young driver accidents in the UK: The influence of age, experience, and time of day. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38, 871-878.

10.

Deffenbacher, J. L., Huff, M. E., Lynch, R. S., Oetting, E. R., & Salvatore, N. F. (2000). Characteristics and treatment of high-anger drivers. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 47, 5-17.

11.

Deffenbacher, J. L., Lynch, R. S., Oetting, E. R., & Yingling, D. A. (2001). Driving anger: correlates and a test of state-trait theory. Personality and Individual Difference, 31, 1321-1331.

12.

Deffenbacher, J. L., Oetting, E. R., & Lynch, R. S. (1994). Development of a driving anger scale. Psychological Reports, 74, 83-91.

13.

DeLucia, P. R., Kathryn Bleckley, M., Meyer, L. E., & Bush, J. M. (2003). Judgments about collision in younger and older drivers. Transportation Research part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 6, 63-80.

14.

Ebbesen, E. B., Parker, S., & Konecni, V. J. (1977). Laboratory and field analyses of decisions involving risk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 576-589.

15.

Gibson, J. J. (1961). The contribution of experimental psychology to the formulation of the problem of safety-A brief for basic research. In Behavioral approaches to accident research(pp. 77-89). New York: Association for the Aid of Crippled Children.

16.

Goldenbeld, C., Levelt, P. B. M., & Heidstra, J. (2000). Psychological perspectives on changing driver attitude and behaviour. Recherche Transports-Sécurité, 67, 65-81.

17.

Gray, R., & Regan, D. M. (2005). Perceptual processes used by drivers during overtaking in a driving simulator. Human Factors, 47, 394-417.

18.

Groeger, J. A. (2000). Understanding driving: Applying cognitive psychology to a complex everyday task. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

19.

Halpern, D. F. (1992). Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

20.

Hegeman, G. (2004). Overtaking frequency and advanced driver assistance systems. In Proceedings of the IEEE Intelligent Vehicles Symposium, 14-17. Parma, Italy.

21.

Hill, J. D., & Salzman, J. A. (2012). Enhancing speed Pprception in virtual environments through training. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 56, 1772-1776.

22.

Hills, B. L. (1980). Vision, visibility, and perception in driving. Perception, 9, 183-216.

23.

Hoyle, F. (1957). The black cloud. Middlesex, UK: Penguin.

24.

Jeffcoat, G. O., Skelton, N., & Smeed, R. J. (1973). Analysis of national statistics of overtaking accidents. London: University of London, International Driver Behavior Research Association.

25.

Kietzmann, T. C., & König, P. (2015). Effects of contextual information and stimulus ambiguity on overt visual sampling behavior. Vision research, 110, 76-86.

26.

Konecni, V. J., Ebbesen, E. B., & Konecni, D. K. (1976). Decision processes and risk taking in traffic: Driver response to the onset of yellow light. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 359-367.

27.

Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2004). Detection theory: A user's guide. Psychology press.

28.

Massie, D. L., Campbell, K. L., & Williams, A. F. (1995). Traffic accident involvement rates by driver age and gender. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 27, 73-87.

29.

McLeod, R. W., & Ross, H. E. (1983). Optic-flow and cognitive factors in time-to-collision estimates. Perception, 12, 417-423.

30.

OECD (1999). Safety strategies for rural roads. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development, Paris.

31.

Özkan, T., & Lajunen, T. (2005). Why are there sex differences in risky driving? The relationship between sex and gender‐role on aggressive driving, traffic offences, and accident involvement among young Turkish drivers. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 547-558.

32.

Raghuram, A., & Lakshminarayanan, V. (2011). Psychophysical estimation of the effects of aging on direction-of-heading judgments. Journal of Modern Optics, 58, 1837-1847.

33.

Regan, D., & Beverley, K. I. (1978). Looming detectors in the human visual pathway. Vision Research, 18, 415-421.

34.

Regan, D., & Beverley, K. I. (1979). Visually guided locomotion: Psychophysical evidence for a neural mechanism sensitive to flow patterns. Science, 205, 311-313.

35.

Schiff, W., & Detwiler, M. L. (1979). Information used in judging impending collision. Perception, 8, 647-658.

36.

Schiff, W., & Oldak, R. (1990). Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 303.

37.

Toledo-Moreo, R., Santa, J., & Zamora-Izquierdo, M. A. (2009). A cooperative overtaking assistance system. Iros 2009 3rd workshop: Planning, perception and Navigation for Intelligent Vehicles, 50-55.

38.

Wickens, C. D., Hollands, J. G., Banbury, S., & Parasuraman, R. (2015). Engineering psychology & human performance. Psychology Press.

한국심리학회지: 일반