바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

  • ENGLISH
  • P-ISSN1229-067X
  • E-ISSN2734-1127
  • KCI

특성불안과 시간거리 지각이 대안의 선호도에 미치는 영향: 해석수준이론에 근거하여

Effects of Trait Anxiety and Perceived Temporal Distance on Preference: Based on Construal Level Theory

한국심리학회지: 일반 / Korean Journal of Psychology: General, (P)1229-067X; (E)2734-1127
2010, v.29 no.4, pp.659-678
김경욱 (한양대학교)

초록

본 연구는 해석수준이론(construal level theory)에 근거하여 시간거리지각의 개인차가 대안의 선호도에 미치는 영향을 살펴보고, 특성불안과 해석수준과의 관계 및 특성불안이 대안의 선호도에 미치는 영향을 시간거리지각이 조절하는지 알아보고자 수행되었다. 이를 위해 피험자에게 ‘다음 학기’라는 동일한 시간거리를 제시해주고 이를 가깝게 지각하는 집단과 멀리 지각하는 집단으로 나누어 상위수준이 긍정적이지만 하위수준이 부정적인 대안(이하 상위긍정-하위부정인 대안)과 상위수준이 부정적이지만 하위수준이 긍정적인 대안(이하 상위부정-하위긍정인 대안)에 대한 선호도와 상하위수준의 중요도를 평정하게 하였다. 연구결과, 시간거리를 가깝게 지각할 때는 상위수준보다 하위수준의 정보를 더 중요하게 평가하는 반면, 시간거리를 멀리 지각할 때는 하위수준보다 상위수준의 정보를 더 중요하게 평가하는 것으로 나타났다. 또한 시간거리를 가깝게 지각할 때는 상위긍정-하위부정인 대안보다 상위부정-하위긍정인 대안의 선택의도가 높게 나타났다. 특성불안과 해석수준과의 관계를 살펴본 결과, 특성불안이 높을수록 하위수준의 해석을 하는 것으로 나타났으며, 특성불안이 높은 사람들은 시간거리를 가깝게 지각할수록 하위수준을 더 중요하게 평가하고 긍정적 사건의 발생가능성을 더 낮게 예측하는 것으로 나타났다. 특히 상위긍정-하위부정인 대안에 대하여 시간거리 지각에 따른 선호도의 차이가 큰 것으로 나타나 시간거리 지각의 조절효과를 확인할 수 있었다. 이러한 결과를 바탕으로 본 연구의 의의와 시사점을 논의하고, 제한점과 후속연구의 필요성을 제안하였다.

keywords
construal level theory, perceived temporal distance, trait anxiety, preference of the option, 해석수준이론, 시간거리지각, 특성불안, 대안의 선호도

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to investigate effects of perceived temporal distance on preference based on construal level theory(CLT). This study also intended to examine the relationship between trait anxiety and construal level, and perceived temporal distance has a moderating effect in the relationship between trait anxiety and preference of the option. Participants were presented with ‘next semester’ as a criteria to measure individual differences in perceiving temporal distance. The results were as follows. Participants perceiving next semester as near future evaluated low-level related information as more important than high-level related information, in contrast, participants who perceived next semester as distant future considered high-level related information as more important. Further, they prefer the option that has negative high-level feature and positive low-level feature when they perceived next semester as near future. Trait anxiety correlated with construal level negatively. Those with higher trait anxiety evaluated low-level related information as more important, predicted the possibility of positive event as lower when they perceived next semester as near future. Especially it can be suggested that perceived temporal distance was an important factor in moderating the relationship between trait anxiety and the threatening option. The implications and limitations of the study and suggestions for future research are discussed.

keywords
construal level theory, perceived temporal distance, trait anxiety, preference of the option, 해석수준이론, 시간거리지각, 특성불안, 대안의 선호도

참고문헌

1.

김성은 (2009). 해석수준, 인지욕구 및 시간적 거리에 따른 소비자의 정보탐색과정. 이화여자대학교 대학원 석사학위 논문.

2.

김영조 (2006). 시간적 거리에 의한 소비자 정보처리가 제품 선호도에 미치는 영향. 고려대학교 대학원 박사학위 논문.

3.

윤지현 (2007). 불확실성과 시간적 거리가 대안의 해석수준에 미치는 영향. 서강대학교 석사학위 논문.

4.

정환호 (2008). 추론수준 프레이밍 광고형태와 시간거리가 자기통제에 미치는 영향. 서울대학교 석사학위논문.

5.

한덕웅, 이창호, 탁진국 (1993). Spielberger 상태-특성 불안 검사의 표준화. 학생지도연구, 10, 214-222.

6.

Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. M. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuro Psychological Society, 2, 249-255.

7.

Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety Disorders and Phobias: A Cognitive Perspective. New York: Basic Books.

8.

Butler, G., & Mathews, A. (1983). Cognitive process in anxiety. Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, 5, 51-62.

9.

Butler, G., & Mathews, A. (1987). Anticipatory anxiety and risk perception. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 551-565.

10.

Clore, G. L., & Gasper, K. (2001). Feeling is believing: Some affective influences on belief. In N. Frijda, T. Manstead, & S. Bem (Eds.), Emotions and Beliefs (pp.10-44). New York: Cambridge University Press.

11.

Clore, G. L., Gasper, K., & Garvin, E. (2001). Affect-as-information. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition (pp.121-144). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

12.

Coles, M. E., & Heimberg, R. G. (2002). Memory biases in the anxiety disorders: current status. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 587-627.

13.

Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C., & Isbell, L. (2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of Mood and Cognition: A User's Guide (pp.27-62). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

14.

Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 745-761.

15.

Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focusing of attention. In P. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The Heart's Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention (pp.167-192). New York: Academic.

16.

Eyal, T., Sagristano, M. D., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Chaiken, S. (2009). When values matter: Expressing values in behavioral intentions for the near vs. distant future. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 35-43.

17.

Eysenck, M. W. (1997). Anxiety and Cognition: A Unified Theory. Hove: Erlbaum.

18.

Fujita, K., Henderson, M., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Spatial distance and mental construal of social events. Psychological Science, 17, 278-282.

19.

Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal fo Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351-367.

20.

Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34-40.

21.

Henderson, M., Fujita, K., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Transcending the “Here”: The effect of spatial distance on social judgment. Journal fo Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 845-856.

22.

Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206-1217.

23.

Kim, H., & John, D. R. (2008). Consumer response to brand extensions: Construal level as a moderator of the importance of perceived fit. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18, 116-126.

24.

Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5-18.

25.

Maner, J. K., Richey, J. A., Cromer, K., Mallott M., Lejuez, C. W., Joiner, T. E., & Schmidt, N. B. (2007). Dispositional anxiety and risk-avoidant decision-making. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 665-675.

26.

Maner, J. K., & Schmidt, N. B. (2006). The role of risk avoidance in anxiety. Behaviour Therapy, 37, 181-189.

27.

Nussbaum, S., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Creeping dispositionism: The temporal dynamics of behavior prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 485-497.

28.

Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision-making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 79, 56-77.

29.

Sagristano, M. D., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2002). Time dependent gambling: Odds now, money later. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 364-376.

30.

Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Anxiety as an emotional state. In C. D. Spielberger(Ed.), Anxiety: Current Trends in Theory and Research. New York: Academic Press.

31.

Stӧber, J. (1997). Trait anxiety and pessimistic appraisal of risk and chance. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 465-476.

32.

Todorov, A., Goren, A., & Trope, Y. (2007). Probability as a psychological distance: Construal and preference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 473-482.

33.

Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2000). Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 876-889.

34.

Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403-421.

35.

Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1985). A Theory of Action Identification. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

36.

Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3-15.

37.

Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 660-671.

38.

Vallacher, R. R., Wegner, D. M., & Frederick, J. (1987). The presentation of self through action identification. Social Cognition, 5, 301-322.

39.

Wakslak, C. J., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Alony, R. (2006). Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: Probability and the mental representation of events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 641-653.

40.

Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders(2nd ed.), Chichester: Wiley.

한국심리학회지: 일반