바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

  • ENGLISH
  • P-ISSN1229-067X
  • E-ISSN2734-1127
  • KCI

시각적 자극의 정서가와 정서강도에 따른 동공크기 변화

Pupillary Responses as an Indication of Emotional Arousal

한국심리학회지: 일반 / Korean Journal of Psychology: General, (P)1229-067X; (E)2734-1127
2013, v.32 no.4, pp.829-844
맹세호 (가톨릭대학교)
정윤경 (가톨릭대학교)
이중 (국립과학수사연구원)

초록

동공크기의 변화는 활성화된 심리적 상태, 혹은 정서적 각성을 측정하는데 유용하기 때문에 여러 연구자들에 의해 심리적 활동 과정을 이해하는 지표로 사용되었다. 본 연구는 시각적 자극의 긍정적, 부정적. 중립적 정서가(emotional valence)에 따른 동공크기의 차이를 탐색하고 자극의 객관적, 주관적 정서강도와 동공크기 사이의 관계를 살펴보기 위하여 설계되었다. 이를 위하여 성인 실험참가자 102명(남 28, 여 74)을 대상으로 International Affective Picture System(IAPS: Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2008)에 의해 평정된 정서자극을 제시하고 동공크기의 변화를 측정하고 각 자극의 강도에 대한 주관적 경험을 보고하도록 하였다. 연구 결과, 정서가에 따라 동공크기의 변화가 유의미한 것으로 나타났다. 사후검증 결과 부정적, 긍정적 정서가를 가진 자극과 중립적 자극 사이의 동공크기는 유의미한 차이를 나타냈으나 긍정적 자극과 부정적 자극 사이의 동공크기 변화는 유의미하지 않았다. 정서강도(객관적/주관적)와 동공크기의 상관분석 결과 자극의 객관적 정서강도와 주관적 정서강도가 동공크기와 정적관계를 가지는 것을 검증하여 정서적 자극에 대한 주관적 경험의 강도가 증가함에 따라 동공의 크기가 확장됨을 확인하였다.

keywords
동공크기, 정서가. 정서강도, 정서적 각성, 자율신경계의 활동, pupillary dilation, emotional valence. emotional intensity, emotional arousal, activation of autonomic nervous system

Abstract

Pupillary dilation has been used for understanding the process of psychological activity by several researchers. The present study was aimed to find difference in pupillary dilation according to emotional valence of visual stimuli and to reveal relationship between emotional intensity and pupillary dilation. 102 subjects’(28 males, 74 females) pupil responses were measured while presenting 10 negative, 10 neutral and 10 positive pictures. The subjects also rated their subjective emotional intensity that they experienced while they were exposed to the stimuli. Our results showed that the amount of pupillary dilation was significantly different according to the valence of emotional stimuli. We found significant difference in pupillary dilation between neutral stimuli and stimuli with emotional valence(negative/positive). But there was no significant difference between positive and negative stimuli. Most importantly, there was significant positive relation between emotional intensity(objective/subjective) and pupillary dilation. These results suggests that pupillary dilation may indicate the level of emotional arousal that a subject experiences.

keywords
동공크기, 정서가. 정서강도, 정서적 각성, 자율신경계의 활동, pupillary dilation, emotional valence. emotional intensity, emotional arousal, activation of autonomic nervous system

참고문헌

1.

김영미 (2010). 디지털 미디어 인터페이스에서 상호작용 속성과 사용자 경험의 관계성에 관한 연구. 이화여자대학교 박사학위 논문.

2.

김지은 (2004). 자율신경활성도 평가를 위한 동공 영상시스템 개발. 경희대학교 석사학위 논문.

3.

김희송, 이 중, 홍현기, 박병욱, 서중석 (2011). 적외선을 이용한 동공 측정 시스템의 개발 한국법과학회. 11(2), 40-43.

4.

박태진, 박선희. (2009). Iaps 자극에 대한 한국 대학생의 정서 평가. 인지과학, 20(2), 183- 195.

5.

석현정, 정상훈, 박종민, 박진영, & 정은빛. (2007). 사진 이미지의 색조 변화에 따른 감성적 반응연구. 디자인학연구, 20(5), 229-238.

6.

윤현중 (2011). 감성 컴퓨팅 기술을 이용한 자폐 진단/치료 시스템. 자연과학연구논문집, 9(1), 1-7.

7.

윤현중, 정성엽 (2010). 자폐스펙트럼장애 진단 및 치료를 위한 감성 컴퓨팅 기술 동향 분석. 감성과학, 13(3), 429-440.

8.

이경화, 이임갑, 손진훈 (1999) 정서의 심리적, 생리적 측정 및 지표개발: 기본정서 구분모델, 한국감성과학회 춘계학술대회논문집, 2, 43-52.

9.

장은혜, 손진훈 (2009). 시청각 ‘공포’ 동영상 자극이 뇌 활동과 말초신경계 반응에 미치는 효과. 충남대학교 박사학위 논문.

10.

Bernhardt, P. C., Dabbs, J. M., Riad, J. K., 1996. Pupillometry system for use in social psychology. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 28(1), 61-66.

11.

Bram C., G. (1972). Psychological significance of pupillary movements. Psychological Bulletin, 77(5), 340-355.

12.

Bradley MM, Miccoli L, Escrig MA, Lang PJ (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 602-607.

13.

Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., 1999. International affective digitized sounds (IADS): stimuli, instructionmanual and affective ratings. Technical Report B-2, University of Florida, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, FL.

14.

Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp.173-191). New York: Guilford Press.

15.

Calcagnini, G., Lino, S., Censi, F., & Cerutti, S. (1997). Cardiovascular autonomic rhythms in spontaneous pupil fluctuations. Computers in Cardiology, 199, 133-136.

16.

Calcagnini, G., Censi, F., Lino, S., & Cerutti, S. (2000). Pupil diameter variability in humans. Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 2000. Proceedings of the 22nd Annual International Conference of the IEEE, 3, 2298-2301.

17.

Causse, M., Sénard, J., Démonet, J., Pastor, J., (2010). Monitoring cognitive and emotional processes through pupil and cardiac response during dynamic versus logical task. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 35(2), 115-123.

18.

Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biological Psychology, 67(1-2), 7-50.

19.

DeBoer, R. W., Karemaker, J. M., Strackee, J., (1984). Comparing spectra of a series of point events particularly for heart-rate-variability data, IEEE Trans. Bio-Med. Eng. 31, 384.

20.

Goldwater, B. C. (1972). Psychological significance of pupillary movements. Psychological Bulletin, 77(5), 340-355.

21.

Granholm, E., & Steinhauer, S. R. (2004). Pupillometric measures of cognitive and emotional processes. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 52(1), 1-6.

22.

Hess, E. H. & Polt, J. M. (1960). Pupil size as related to interest value of visual stimuli. Science, 132, 349-350.

23.

Hess, E. H. (1960). Attitude and pupil size. Scientific American, 212(4), 46-54.

24.

Hess, E. H., Petrovich, S. B., (1987). Pupillary behavior in communication. In: Siegman, A. W., Feldstein, S. (Eds.), Nonverbal Behavior and Communication, (pp.327-348) Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

25.

Hoeks, B., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1993). Pupillary dilation as a measure of attention: A quantitative system analysis. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc, 25(1), 16.

26.

Jainta, S., & Baccino, T. (2010). Analyzing the pupil response due to increased cognitive demand: An independent component analysis study. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 77(1), 1-7.

27.

J. McNames, T. Thong, and B. Goldstein, (2003). Reliability and accuracy of heart rate variability metrics versus ECG segment duration, in Proceedings of the 25th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering inMedicine and Biology Society.

28.

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

29.

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.

30.

Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1987). Affective intensity as an individual difference characteristic: A review. Journal of Research in Personality, 21(1), 1-39.

31.

LeDoux JE, Phelps EA (2000). Emotional networks in the brain. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM (eds) Handbook of emotions, (pp.157-172). Guilford, New York.

32.

Libby, W. L., Lacey, B. C., & Lacey, J. I. (1973). Pupillary and cardiac activity during visual attention. Psychophysiology, 10(3), 270-294.

33.

M. P. Tarvainen, A. S. Koistinen, M. Valkonen- Korhonen, J. Partanen, and P. A. Karjalainen, (2001). Analysis of galvanic skin responses with principal components and clustering techniques, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 48(10), 1071-1079.

34.

Nunnally, J., Knott, P., Duchnowski, A., & Parker, R. (1967). Pupillary response as a general measure of activation Springer New York. doi: 10.3758/BF03210310

35.

Partala, T. and Surakka, V. (2003) Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. International J. of Human-Computer Studies 59(1-2), 185-198.

36.

Rizzo, A. A., Klimchuk, D., Mitura, R., Bowerly, T., Buckwalter, G. B., Kerns, K., Randall, K., Adams, R., Finn, P., Tarnanas, I., Sirbu, C., Ollendick, T. H., & Yeh, S. C. (2005). A virtual reality scenario for all seasons: The virtual classroom, Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Human Computer Interaction, Las Vegas, USA, July.

37.

Steinhauer, S. R., Boller, F., Zubin, J., & Pearlman, S. (1983). Pupillary dilation to emotional visual stimuli revisited. Psychophysiology, 20(4), 472. [Abstract]

38.

Steinhauer, S. R., Siegle, G. J., Condray, R., and Pless, M. (2004). Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 52(1), 77-86

39.

Surwillo, W. W. (1969). Statistical distribution of volar skin potential level in attention and the effects of age. Psychophysiology, 6(1), 13-16.

40.

Tryon, W. W. (1975). Pupillometry: A survey of sources of variation. Psychophysiology, 12(1), 90-93.

41.

Williams, L.A., Das, P., Liddell, B., Olivieri, G., Peduto, A., Brammer, M., Gordon, E., (2005). BOLD, sweat and fears: fMRI and skin conductance distinguish facial fear signals. NeuroReport 16(1), 49-52.

한국심리학회지: 일반