바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

Korean Journal of Psychology: General

  • KOREAN
  • P-ISSN1229-067X
  • E-ISSN2734-1127
  • KCI

Children's Perceptual Capacity to Detect Collision Impact

Korean Journal of Psychology: General / Korean Journal of Psychology: General, (P)1229-067X; (E)2734-1127
2013, v.32 no.2, pp.325-349



Abstract

Two experiments investigated children’s perceptual capacity to detect potential collision impacts. Children from 4 to 12 years of age participated as observers in the study. In Experiment 1, displays depicted either a small car or a large truck approaching the observer against a road-scene background, producing a local perturbation in the visual field. In Experiment 2 displays depicted the observer’s own movement toward obstacles (a global perturbation of the visual field). Simulated approaches were created following the tau-dot hypothesis in which, when tau-dot ≥ -0.5, approaches result in safe stops without collision; but when tau-dot < -0.5, approaches result in collisions with impact. Predefined tau-dot values remained constant throughout each simulated approach. Results demonstrated that 4-6 year olds performed poorly compared with 7-12 year olds. Nevertheless, even the 4 year olds performed consistently with that predicted by the tau-dot hypothesis in Experiment 1 but their performance deteriorated to chance level in Experiment 2. Current child pedestrian safety education focuses on facilitating children’s abilities to cross streets safely by enhancing their sensitivity to optical variables specifying time-to-contact. This research supports developing children’s perceptual capacity to detect potential collision impact as part of these training programs.

keywords
Child pedestrian safety, street crossing, tau-dot, collision impact, time-to-contact, traffic safety education, 어린이 보행자 교통사고, 도로횡단, tau-dot, 충돌탐지, 접촉시간, 교통안전교육

Reference

1.

도로교통공단 (2010). 교통사고 분석 자료집. 서울: 경신기획.

2.

김남균 (2010). 성긴 광흐름 속에서 접촉시간 지각. 한국심리학회: 인지 및 생물, 22, 129 -145.

3.

Ampofo-Boateng, K., Thomson, J. A., Grieve, R., Pitcairn, T., Lee, D. N., & Demetre, J. D. (1991). A developmental and training study of children’s ability to find safe routes to cross the road. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 31-45.

4.

Andersen, G. J., Cisneros, J., Atchley, P., & Saidpour, A. (1999). Speed, size and edge-rate information for the detection of collision events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 256-269.

5.

Benguigui, N., Broderick, M. P., Baurès, R., & Amorim, M.-A. (2008). Motion prediction and the velocity effect in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 389-407.

6.

Bootsma, R. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1990). Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 21-29.

7.

Caird, J. K., & Hancock, P. A. (1994). The perception of arrival time for different oncoming vehicles at an intersection. Ecological Psychology, 6, 83-109.

8.

Connelly, M. L., Conaglen, H. M., Parsonson, B. S., & Isler, R. B. (1998). Child pedestrians’ crossing gap thresholds. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 30, 443-453.

9.

Cross, R. T., & Mehegan, J. (1988). Young children’s conception of speed: Possible implications for pedestrian safety. International Journal of Science Education, 10, 253-265.

10.

Demetre, J. D., Lee, D. N., Pitcairn, T. K., Grieve, R., Thomson, J. A., & Ampofo-Boateng, K. (1992). Errors in young children’s decisions about traffic gaps: experiments with roadside simulations. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 189-202.

11.

Department for Transport. (2010). Pedestrian casualties in reported road accidents 2008: Road Accident Statistics Factsheet No. 3. Retrieved January 10, 2013, from http://www. dft.gov.uk/pgr/statistics/datatablespublications/accidents/casualtiesgba/suppletablesfactsheets/pedestrian2008.pdf

12.

Duperrex, O., Bunn, F., & Roberts, I. (2002). Safety education of pedestrians for injury prevention: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 324, 1145-1148.

13.

Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1983) A review of some Piagetian concepts. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of Child Development (Vol. III, pp.167-230). New York: Wiley.

14.

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

15.

Gray, R., & Regan, D. (1998). Accuracy of estimating time to collision using binocular and monocular information. Vision Research, 38, 499-512.

16.

Hoffman, E. R., Payne, A. & Prescott, S. (1980). Children’s estimates of vehicle approach times. Human Factors, 22, 235-240.

17.

Keshavara, B., Landwehr, K., Baures, R., Oberfeld, D., & Hecht, H. (2010). Age-correlated incremental consideration of velocity information in relative time-to-arrival Judgment. Ecology Psychology, 22, 212-221.

18.

Kim, N.-G., & Grocki, M. J. (2006). Multiple sources of information and time-to-contact judgments. Vision Research, 46, 1946-1958.

19.

Kim, N.-G., & Turvey, M. T. (1998). Optical flow fields and Berstein's “modeling of the future,” In M. T. Latash (Ed.), Progress in motor control (pp.221-265). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

20.

Kim, N.-G., Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (1993). Optical information about the severity of upcoming contacts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 179-193.

21.

Kuhn, D. (1988) Cognitive development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental Psychology: An Advanced Textbook. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

22.

Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to- collision. Perception, 5, 437-459.

23.

Lee, D. N., Young, D. S., & McLaughlin, C. M. (1984). A roadside simulation of road crossing for children. Ergonomics, 27, 1271-1281.

24.

Lee, D. N., Young, D. S., & Rewt. D. (1992). How do somersaulters land on their feet? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1195-1202.

25.

Malek, M., Guyer, B. & Lescohier, I. (1990). The epidemiology and prevention of child pedestrian injury. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 22, 301-313.

26.

Michon, J. A. (1981) Traffic education for young pedestrians: an introduction. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 13, 163-167.

27.

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (2010). Traffic Safety Facts: 2009 Data. Retrieved January 10, 2013, from http:// www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/Pubs/811387.pdf

28.

Piaget, J. (1969). The child's concept of time. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

29.

Rock, P. B., & Harris, M. G. (2006) as a potential control variable for visually guided braking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 251-267.

30.

Rothengatter, J. A. (1984). A behavioural approach to improving traffic behaviour of young children. Ergonomics, 2, 147-160.

31.

Routledge, D. A., Repetto-Wright, R., & Howarth, C. I. (1974). The exposure of young children to accident risk as pedestrians. Ergonomics, 17, 457-480.

32.

Sandels, S. (1975). Children in traffic. London: Elek.

33.

Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Whiting, H. T. A., & Bootsma, R. J. (1991). “Grasping” tau! Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 315-33.

34.

Schieber, R. A., & Vegega, M. E. (2002). Reducing childhood pedestrian injuries: Summary of a multidisciplinary conference. Injury Prevention, 8 (Suppl. 1), i1-10.

35.

Smith, M. R, H., Flach, J. M., Dittman, S. M., & Stanard, T. (2001). Monocular optical constraints on collision control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 395-410.

36.

Struik, M., Alexander, K., Cave, T., Fleming, A., Lyttle, J., & Stone, A. (1988). Pedestrian accident project report No. 4, Literature review of factors contributing to pedestrian accidents. Melbourne: Road Traffic Authority.

37.

Thomson, J. (1996). Child pedestrian accidents: what makes children vulnerable? In B. Gillam, & J. Thomson (Eds.). Child Safety: Problems and Prevention from Pre-school to Adolescence. London: Routledge.

38.

Thomson, J. A., Tolmie, A. K., Foot, H. C., Whelan, K. M., SArvary, P., & Morrison, S. (2005). Influence of virtual reality training on the roadside crossing judgments of child pedestrian. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 175-186.

39.

Thomson, J. A., & Whelan, K. M. (1997). A community approach to road safety education using practical training methods: The Drumchapel project (Road Safety Research Rep. No. 3). London: Department of the Environment, Transport & the Regions.

40.

Tolmie, A. K., Thomson, J. A., Foot, H. C., Burkes, M., Wu, C., Towner, E. L. M., et al. (2003). Training children in the safe use of designated crossings (Road Safety Research Rep. No. 34). London: Department for Transport.

41.

Toroyan, T., & Peden, M. (2007). Youth and Road Safety. Geneva: World Health Organization.

42.

Treffner, P., Barrett, R., & Petersen, A. (2002). Stability and skill in driving. Human Movement Science, 21, 749-784.

43.

van der Kamp, J., Savelsbergh, G., & Smeets, J. (1997). Multiple information sources in interceptive timing. Human Movement Science, 16, 780-821.

44.

Vinjé, M. P. (1981). Children as pedestrians: abilities and limitations. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 13, 225-240.

45.

Wann, J. P., Poulter, D. R., & Purcell, C. (2011). Reduced sensitivity to visual looming inflates the risk posed by speeding vehicles when children try to cross the road. Psychological Science, 22, 429-434.

46.

Whitebread, D., & Neilson, K. (2000). The contribution of visual search strategies to the development of pedestrian skills by 4-11 years old children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 539-557.

47.

World Health Organization (2006). Road traffic injury prevention: training manual. WHO.

48.

Yilmaz, E. H., & Warren, W. H. (1995). Visual control of braking: a test of the tau hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 996 -1014.

49.

Zeedyk, S., Wallace, L., Carcary, B., Jones, K., & Larter, K. (2001). Children and road safety: increasing knowledge does not improve behaviour. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 573-587.

Korean Journal of Psychology: General