ISSN : 1226-9654
To examine properties of visual working memory (VWM) representations, event-related potentials were measured during the comparison process between memory and perception in a change-detection task. Subjects were asked to detect an orientation change upon a memory display onset after remembering a few orientation bars, and the memory set size was varied from 1 to 6. Onset latencies of attention shift triggered by a memory-perception difference were measured by N2pc component onset time, and the latencies were constant regardless of the number of items in the display. The results indicate that the comparison between VWM representations and perceptual inputs occurs very rapidly and is accomplished immediately upon a test display onset, and further that the representations in VWM are stored into 3-4 fixed-resolution slots.
현주석 (2008a). 시각적 탐색에서 표적에 의해 유발된 N2pc 성분의 특성 및 측정. 한국심리학회지: 실험, 20(4), 247-263.
현주석 (2008b). 차폐 자극이 시각 작업 기억 비교 과정에 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회지: 실험, 20(3), 167-178.
Agam, Y., Hyun, J. -S., Danker, J. F., Zhou F., Kahana, M., & Sekuler, R. (2009). Early neural signature of visual short-term memory. NeuroImage, 44(2). 531-536.
Alvarez, G. A., & Cavanagh, P. (2004). The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by information load and by number of objects. Psychological Science, 15, 106-111.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Clarendon.
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87-185.
Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 225-234.
Hyun, J.-S., Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2006). How change-detection is related to visual search: A change in a remembered object is like a simple feature. [Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 6(6), 985a.
Hyun, J.-S., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 154-158.
Hyun, J.-S., Woodman, G. F., Vogel, E. K., Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2009). The comparison of visual working memory representations with perceptual inputs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1140-1160.
Hyun, J.-S., Woodman, G. F., Vogel, E. K., Niese, A. T., & Luck, S. J. (2003). How are visual inputs compared with memory representations in the change-detection paradigm? [Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 3(9), 322a.
Jennings, J. R., & Wood, C. C. (1976). The e-adjustment procedure for repeated-measures analyses of variance. Psychophysiology, 13, 277-278.
Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and Performance IX (pp.187-203). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology, 38, 557-577.
Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 476-490.
Luck, S. J. (2005). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Luck, S. J., & Ford, M. A. (1998). On the role of selective attention in visual perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 95, 825-830.
Luck, S. J., Girelli, M., McDermott, M. T., & Ford, M. A. (1997). Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: An ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 64-87.
Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Electrophysiological evidence for parallel and serial processing during visual search. Perception and Psychophysics, 48, 603-617.
Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology, 31, 291-308.
Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279-281.
Luck, S. J., Woodman, G. F., & Vogel, E. K. (2000). Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 432-440.
Mueller, H. J., & Rabbit, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315-330.
Nakayama, K., & Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vision Research, 29, 1631-1647.
Verleger, R. (1997). On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. Psychophysiology, 34, 131-156.
Vogel, E. K., & Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature, 428, 748-751.
Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Storage of features, conjunctions, and objects in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 92-114.
Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature, 400, 867-869.
Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2003). Serial deployment of attention during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 121-138.
Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2003). Slot-like versus continuous representations in visual working memory. Journal of Vision, 3, 681a.
Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2008). Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory. Nature, 453, 233-235.