바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

logo

Working Memory Training Effects According to Domain-Specificity: A Meta-Analysis

The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology / The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, (P)1226-9654; (E)2733-466X
2014, v.26 no.4, pp.207-231
https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2014.26.4.001


  • Downloaded
  • Viewed

Abstract

Working memory(WM) plays an important role in various cognitive tasks. Accordingly, WM training programs have been developed in order to enhance cognitive functions. However, it has been under debate on the effects of the training program. In this study, we performed meta-analyses to test whether WM training programs can improve other cognitive functions based on the domain-specificity of WM. In doing so, we classified previous studies as “congruent” or “incongruent” according to whether the domains of training programs were consistent with those of the measurement tools. As categorized general cognitive task involving measurement task about attention, inhibition, verbal ability and intelligence as well as WM task according to domain specificity, we examined improvement of general cognitive ability when the specific WM domain in the cognitive function was trained. Then, effect sizes were calculated based on statistics reported in the previous studies and meta-analyses were conducted in consideration of measurement periods and participants characteristics. Additionally, Chi square analyses were performed in order to confirm that the domain-specificity could be an important variable in the training effects. The results showed that domain congruency had a great influence on the training effects. The effect sizes were greater when training and measurement domains were consistent than when those were inconsistent, for both of the immediate and follow-up measurements. In addition, the effect of the congruency was greater in the patient groups compared to the normal groups. These findings suggest that the domain-specificity of WM training is an important factor in deciding the effects of the WM training programs.

keywords
working memory, working memory training, domain-specificity, 작업기억, 작업기억 훈련, 영역 특수성

Reference

1.

김지연, & 백용매 (2007). 정상아동과 주의력결핍 과잉행동장애아동의 기억결함과 실행기능의 차이. 상담학연구, 8(1), 333-349.

2.

박찬흠, & 이형청 (2009). 작업기억에서의 영역 제한적인 저장 및 처리와 영역 일반적인 영역간 처리. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 21(1), 1-21.

3.

신경희, & 김초복 (2013). 대상, 공간 및 언어 인지양식에 따른 작업기억 과제 수행의 개인차. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 25(4), 539-563.

4.

Alloway, T. P., Bibile, V., & Lau, G. (2014). Computerized working memory training: Can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? (vol 29, pg 632, 2012). Computers in Human Behavior, 34, 352-352.

5.

Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., Willis, C., Eaglen, R., & Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and phonological awareness as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417-426.

6.

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556-559.

7.

Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423.

8.

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829-839.

9.

Brehmer, Y., Rieckmann, A., Bellander, M., Westerberg, H., Fischer, H., & Backman, L. (2011). Neural correlates of training-related working-memory gains in old age. Neuroimage, 58(4), 1110-1120.

10.

Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 246-251.

11.

Cattell, R. B. (1940). A culture-free intelligence test. I. Journal of Educational Psychology, 31(3), 161.

12.

Chein, J. M., Moore, A. B., & Conway, A. R. (2011). Domain-general mechanisms of complex working memory span. Neuroimage, 54(1), 550-559.

13.

Chein, J. M., & Schneider, W. (2005). Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 607-623.

14.

Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(12), 547-552.

15.

Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Keil, K., & Haxby, J. V. (1996). Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6(1), 39-49.

16.

Dang, C. P., Braeken, J., Colom, R., Ferrer, E., & Liu, C. (2014). Why is working memory related to intelligence? Different contributions from storage and processing. Memory, 22(4), 426-441.

17.

Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387- 1388.

18.

Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1-16.

19.

Gooding, D. C., & Tallent, K. A. (2004). Nonverbal working memory deficits in schizophrenia patients: evidence of a supramodal executive processing deficit. Schizophrenia Research, 68(2-3), 189-201.

20.

Gruber, O., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). The functional neuroanatomy of human working memory revisited - Evidence from 3-T fMRl studies using classical domain-specific interference tasks. Neuroimage, 19(3), 797-809.

21.

Haavisto, M. L., & Lehto, J. E. (2005). Fluid/ spatial and crystallized intelligence in relation to domain-specific working memory: A latent- variable approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(1), 1-21.

22.

Hedges, L., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. San Diego: Academic Press.

23.

Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 66- 71.

24.

Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317-324.

25.

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage Publications.

26.

Logie, R. H. (2011). The Functional Organization and Capacity Limits of Working Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(4), 240-245.

27.

Logie, R. H., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S., & Baddeley, A. D. (2004). Is there a specific executive capacity for dual task coordination? Evidence from Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 504-513.

28.

Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(4), 377-384.

29.

McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(3), 240-260.

30.

Melby-Lervag, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is Working Memory Training Effective? A Meta-Analytic Review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270-291.

31.

Nee, D. E., Brown, J. W., Askren, M. K., Berman, M. G., Demiralp, E., Krawitz, A., et al. (2013). A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23(2), 264-282.

32.

Olesen, P. J., Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2004). Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 7(1), 75-79.

33.

Pimperton, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Suppressing irrelevant information from working memory: Evidence for domain-specific deficits in poor comprehenders. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(4), 380-391.

34.

Rapport, M. D., Alderson, R. M., Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Bolden, J., & Sims, V. (2008). Working memory deficits in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the contribution of central executive and subsystem processes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(6), 825-837.

35.

Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 359-379.

36.

Ricciardi, E., Bonino, D., Gentili, C., Sani, L., Pietrini, P., & Vecchi, T. (2006). Neural correlates of spatial working memory in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study comparing visual and tactile processes. Neuroscience, 139(1), 339-349.

37.

Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is Working Memory Training Effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628- 654.

38.

Shiran, A., & Breznitz, Z. (2011). The effect of cognitive training on recall range and speed of information processing in the working memory of dyslexic and skilled readers. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24(5), 524-537.

39.

Sinzig, J., Morsch, D., Bruning, N., Schmidt, M. H., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Inhibition, flexibility, working memory and planning in autism spectrum disorders with and without comorbid ADHD-symptoms. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health, 2(1), 4.

40.

Swanson, L., & Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children's mathematical performance. Intelligence, 35(2), 151-168.

41.

Wang, S. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Working memory deficits in children with reading difficulties: Memory span and dual task coordination. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 188-197.

42.

Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.

43.

Wilson, K. M., & Swanson, H. L. (2001). Are mathematics disabilities due to a domain- general or a domain-specific working memory deficit? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(3), 237-248.

The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology