바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

  • P-ISSN1226-9654
  • E-ISSN2733-466X
  • KCI

불안상태가 초점주의 향상에 의한 방향선택 수행에 미치는 영향

The Influence of an Anxious Mood on Human Directional Choice by Enhancing Focused Attention

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물 / The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, (P)1226-9654; (E)2733-466X
2019, v.31 no.2, pp.181-190
https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2019.31.2.009
김초복 (경북대학교)
이윤지 (경북대학교)

Abstract

The current study aimed to examine how the anxious mood state affects human directional choice. For this purpose, we employed a version of the spatial Stroop task and tested the effect of the induced anxious mood state on the facilitation and interference effects. In an experiment, neutral and anxious picture-and-sound sets were employed to manipulate different moods between two groups. After the mood induction, participants performed a version of the spatial Stroop task that included congruent, neutral, and incongruent trials. The results showed that an anxious mood enhanced the facilitation effect, but reduced the interference effect. In contrast, the conflict adaptation effect was not influenced by the anxious mood. These findings suggest that an anxious mood can accelerate target processing through enhancing focused attention but not inhibitory control. The practical implications for human directional choice in an evacuation situation are discussed.

keywords
the spatial Stroop task, the facilitation effect, the interference effect, an anxious mood, evacuation behavior, 공간 스트룹 과제, 촉진효과, 간섭효과, 불안, 피난행동

참고문헌

1.

Baumgartner, T., Esslen, M., & Jancke, L. (2006). From emotion perception to emotion experience: Emotions evoked by pictures and classical music. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60(1), 34-43.

2.

Birk, J. L., Dennis, T. A., Shin, L. M., & Urry, H. L. (2011). Threat facilitates subsequent executive control during anxious mood. Emotion, 11(6), 1291-1304.

3.

Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14(5), 416-421.

4.

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652.

5.

Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). The International Affective Digitized Sounds (2nd Edition; IADS‐2): Affective Ratings of Sounds and Instruction Manual. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, Gainesville.

6.

Choi, Y., Lee, S., Choi, I. M., Jung, S., Park, Y. K., Kim, C. (2015). International affective digitized sounds in Korea: a cross-cultural adaptation and validation study. Acta Acustica united with Acustica, 101(1), 134-144.

7.

Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 30(2), 343-353.

8.

Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 380-390.

9.

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340-347.

10.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought‐action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313-332.

11.

Glickman, M., & Lamy, D. (2018). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces is contingent on implicit attentional settings. Cognition & Emotion, 32(2), 303-314.

12.

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the Use of Information - Strategic Control of Activation of Responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 480-506.

13.

Gray, J. R. (2004). Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 46-48.

14.

Hodsoll, S., Viding, E., & Lavie, N. (2011). Attentional Capture by Irrelevant Emotional Distractor Faces. Emotion, 11(2), 346-353.

15.

Hristova, P., & Kokinov, B. (2011). Anxiety fosters relational encoding, In B. Kokinov, A. Karmiloff-Smith, N.J. Nersessian (Eds.), Proceedings of the European Conference on cognitive Science: European Perspectives on Cognitive Science, Sofia: New Bulgarian University Press.

16.

Isen, A. M., Niedenthal, P. M., & Cantor, N. (1992). An influence of positive affect on social categorization. Motivation and Emotion, 16(1), 65-78.

17.

Kazen, M., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Intention memory and achievement motivation: Volitional facilitation and inhibition as a function of affective contents of need-related stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 426-448.

18.

Kim, C., Hur, M., Oh, Y., Choi, J. H., & Jeong, J. J. (2016). The Effect of the Running-Man Emergency Exit Sign and Its Installed Location on Human Directional Choice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(6), 1014-1019.

19.

Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1999). Volitional facilitation of difficult intentions: joint activation of intention memory and positive affect removes stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 382.

20.

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 1, 39-58.

21.

Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop Process Dissociations - the Relationship between Facilitation and Interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(2), 219-234.

22.

Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The Influence of Irrelevant Location Information on Performance - a Review of the Simon and Spatial Stroop Effects. Psychonomic Bulletin &Review, 2(2), 174-207.

23.

MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163-203.

24.

Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6(5), 450-452.

25.

Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.

26.

Padmala, S., Bauer, A., & Pessoa, L. (2011.) Negative emotion impairs conflict-driven executive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 192.

27.

Phillips, L. H., Bull, R., Adams, E., & Fraser, L. (2002). Positive Mood and Executive Function: Evidence From Stroop and Fluency Tasks. Emotion, 2(1), 12-22.

28.

Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(1), 383-388.

29.

Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2015). Mood states influence cognitive control: the case of conflict adaptation. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 79(5), 759-772.

30.

Stroop, J. R. (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643.

31.

van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., & Hommel, B. (2009). Reward Counteracts Conflict Adaptation: Evidence for a Role of Affect in Executive Control. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1473-1477.

32.

van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., & Hommel, B. (2010). In the Mood for Adaptation: How Affect Regulates Conflict-Driven Control. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1629-1634.

33.

Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Structure of self-reported current affect: Integration and beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 600-619.

34.

Zwosta, K., Hommel, B., Goschke, T., & Fischer, R. (2013). Mood states determine the degree of task shielding in dual-task performance. Cognition and Emotion, 27(6), 1142-1152.

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물