바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

logo

Perceiving the Time to Contact of Rotating Non-Spherical Objects

The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology / The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, (P)1226-9654; (E)2733-466X
2009, v.21 no.2, pp.73-90
https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2009.21.2.002

Abstract

Are human observers incapable of estimating the time-to-contact (TTC) of a tumbling rugby ball while watching it with a single eye, as Gray and Regan (2000) contend? Everyday experiences suggest otherwise. In Gray and Regan’s study, the oval object rotated only 90 deg so that, for a given trial, its projected shape changed either from circle to ellipse or from ellipse to circle depending on the initial orientation of the object. Thus, despite the fact that an infinite variety of optical patterns can be engendered by rotating non-spherical objects, only two types of deformation were depicted in Gray and Regan’s study. For that reason, additional studies are clearly warranted. The present study was conducted directed at perceptual capacity for estimating the TTC of rotating non-spherical objects. Two different response measures, a relative (Experiment 1) and an absolute (Experiment 2) judgment task, in conjunction with three types of objects, a sphere, a rugby ball shaped object, and a disk shaped object, were employed for this purpose. The objects were depicted as texture-mapped images. Even with the surface texture of the objects, the texture elements projected to the observation point were displaced or even disappeared and were replaced by the texture elements hidden behind due to rotation. Nevertheless, performance was accurate across all conditions of object type. That is to say, participants were as accurate in judging TTC of the two non-spherical objects as they were with the spherical object. Moreover, the effects of velocity and size were also observed, consistent with similar effects reported in other TTC studies. Taken together, the results contradicted Gray and Regan's contention and demonstrated that the human visual system is capable of perceiving TTC of rotating non-spherical objects using information extracted from the surface texture of the objects.

keywords
time-to-contact, local tau, non-spherical objects, surface texture, time-to-contact, local tau, non-spherical objects, surface texture, 접촉시간지각, local tau, 비원형물체, 결 조직

Reference

1.

Andersen, G. J., Cisneros, J., Atchley, P., & Saidpour, A. (1999). Speed, size, and edge-rate information for the detection of collision events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 256-269.

2.

Bennett, S., van der Kamp, J. Savelsbergh, G. J. P., & Davids, K. (1999). Timing a one-handed catch 1. Effects of telestereoscopic viewing. Experimental Brain Research, 129, 362-368.

3.

Bootsma, R. J., & Craig, C. M. (2002). Global and local contributions to the optical specification of time to contact: Observer sensitivity to composite tau. Perception, 31, 901-924.

4.

Bootsma, R. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1990). Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 21-29.

5.

Cavallo, V., & Laurent, M. (1988). Visual information and skill level in time-to-collision estimation. Perception, 17, 623-632.

6.

DeLucia, P. R. (1991). Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 738-748.

7.

DeLucia, P. R. (2005). Does binocular disparity or familiar size information override effects of relative size on judgements of time to contact. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 865-886.

8.

DeLucia, P. R., & Novak, J. B. (1997). Judgments of relative time-to-contact of more than two approaching objects: Toward a method. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 913-928.

9.

DeLucia, P. R. & Warren, R. (1994). Pictorial and motion-based depth information during active control of self-motion: Size-arrival effects on collision avoidance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 783-798.

10.

Flach, J. M., Warren, R., Garness, S. A., Kelly, L., & Stanard, T. (1997). Perception and control of altitude: Splay and depression angles. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1764-1782.

11.

Gray, R., & Regan, D. (1998). Accuracy of estimating time to collision using binocular and monocular information. Vision Research, 38, 499-512.

12.

Gray, R., & Regan, D. (2000). Estimating time to collision with a rotating nonspherical object. Vision Research, 40, 49-63.

13.

Gray, R., & Regan, D. (2004). The use of binocular time-to-contact information. In H. Hecht & G. J. P. Savelsburgh (Eds.), Time-to-contact (pp. 303-325). Amsterdam:Elsevier.

14.

Hecht, H., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2004). Theories of time-to-contact judgment. In H. Hecht & G. J. P. Savelsburgh (Eds.), Time-to-contact (pp. 1-11). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

15.

Heuer, H. (1993). Estimates of time to contact based on changing size and changing target vergence. Perception, 22, 549-53.

16.

Kaiser, M. K., & Mowafy, L. (1993). Optical specification of time-to-passage: Observer's sensitivity to global tau. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 1028-1040.

17.

Kerzel, D., Hecht, H., & Kim, N.-G. (1999). Global expansion, not global tau explains depth from global optic flow. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1540-1555.

18.

Kim, N.-G. (2008). Dynamic occlusion and optical flow from corrugated surfaces. Ecological Psychology, 20, 209-239.

19.

Kim, N.-G. (2009). Sensitivity to local tau's. manuscript submitted for publication.

20.

Kim, N.-G., & Grocki, M. J. (2006). Multiple sources of information and time-to-contact judgments. Vision Research, 46, 1946-1958.

21.

Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5, 437-459.

22.

Lee, D. N., Reddish, P. E., & Rand, D. T. (1991). Aerial docking by hummingbirds. Naturwissenschaften, 78, 526-527.

23.

McLeod, R. W., & Ross, H. E. (1983). Optic-flow and cognitive factors in time-to-collision estimates. Perception, 12, 417-423.

24.

Regan, D. (1997). Visual factors in catching and hitting. Journal of Sports Sciences, 15, 533-558.

25.

Rushton, S. K., & Wann, J. P. (1999). Weighted combination of size and disparity: a computational model for timing a ball catch. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 186-190.

26.

Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Whiting, H. T. A., & Bootsma, R. J. (1991). "Grasping" tau! Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 315-33.

27.

Schiff, W., & Detwiler, M. L. (1979). Information used in judging impending collision. Perception, 8. 647-58.

28.

Scott, M. A., Li, F.-X., & Davids, K. (1996). The shape of things to come: Effects of object shape and rotation on the pick-up of local tau. Ecological Psychology, 8, 343-352.

29.

Smith, M. R, H., Flach, J. M., Dittman, S. M., & Stanard, T. (2001). Monocular optical constraints on collision control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 395-410.

30.

Todd, J. T. (1981). Visual information about moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 795-810.

31.

Tresilian, J. R. (1991). Empirical and theoretical issues in the perception of time to contact. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 865-876.

32.

Tresilian, J. R. (1995). Perceptual and cognitive processes in time-to-contact estimation: Analysis of prediction-motion and relative judgment task. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 231-245.

33.

van der Kamp, J., Savelsbergh, G., & Smeets, J. (1997). Multiple information sources in interceptive timing. Human Movement Science, 16, 780-821.

The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology