바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

중첩된 운동 자극에서 주의에 의한 운동 벡터들 간의 상호 억제

Attention encourages directional suppression in bivectorial motion fields

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물 / The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, (P)1226-9654; (E)2733-466X
2009, v.21 no.2, pp.57-71
https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2009.21.2.001
손원영 (서울대학교)

Abstract

Two experiments were performed to examine attentional suppression in transparently moving dot fields. One of the dot groups were moving coherently in a single direction (effector) and the other were moving randomly (contender). A proportion of the random dot group occasionally moved coherently in the orthogonal direction to the effector during motion adaptation period. In ‘passive’ condition, observers viewed the stimulus without performing any task. In ‘attentive’ condition, they were asked to attend to the occasional coherent motion in the contender by reporting the motion direction. The motion aftereffect for the effector was significantly reduced in the attentive condition compared to that in passive condition. This reduction was present even when the proportion of coherent dots in the contender was zero. The similar results were observed when the occasional coherent motion in the contender was 30 deg apart from the effector, which is well within the range of motion integration. These results show that attention to one component of bivectorial motion results in strong suppression of the unattended component as well as enhancement of the attended one. Such suppression in the small angle difference implies that attention to one of the superimposed motion components encourages segregation between different directions rather than integration.

keywords
attention, bivectorial motion, motion aftereffect, 주의, 중첩된 운동, 운동 잔여 효과

참고문헌

1.

Alais, D. & Blake, R. (1999). Neural strength of visual attention gauged by motion adaptation. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 11, 1015-1018

2.

Braddick, O. and Qian, N. (2001). The organization of global motion and transparency. In Zanker, J. M. & Zeil, J. (Eds.) Motion Vision. Springer- Verlag: Berlin.

3.

Busse, L., Katzner, S., & Treue, S. (2008). Temporal dynamics of neuronal modulation of exogenous and endogenous shifts of visual attention in macaque area MT. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences 105, 16380-16385.

4.

Chaudhuri, A. (1990). Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention. Nature, 344, 60-62.

5.

Day, R. H. & Strelow, E. Reduction of disappearance of visual aftereffect of movement in the absence of patterned surround. Nature, 230, 55-56.

6.

Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18,193–222.

7.

Edwards, M. & Nishida, S. (1999). Global-motion detection with transparent-motion signals.Vision Research 39, 2239-2249.

8.

Graham, N. V. S. (1998). Visual Pattern Analyzers. Oxford University Press. NewYork.

9.

Hiris, E. & Blake, R. (1992). Another perspective on the visual motion aftereffect. Procedings of . National Academy of Science USA, 89, 9025-9028.

10.

Lankheet, M. J. M. & Verstraten, F. A. J. (1995). Attentional modulation of adaptation to two-component transparent motion. Vision Research, 35, 1401-1412.

11.

Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. The Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 2, 467-477.

12.

Martinez-Trujillo, J. & Treue, S. (2002). Attentional modulation strength in cortical area MT depends on stimulus contrast. Neuron.18, 365-370.

13.

Mather, G. & Moulden, B. A. (1980). A simultaneous shift in apparent directions: Further evidence for a ‘distribution-shift’ model of direction coding. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 325-333.

14.

Nakayama, K. & Silverman, G. H. (1986). Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions. Nature, 320, 264-265.

15.

Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision Science: photons to phenomenology. MIT Press.

16.

Raymond, J. E. (2000). Attentional modulation of visual motion perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 42-50.

17.

Raymond, J. E., O'Donnell, H. L., & Tipper, S. P. (1998). Priming reveals attentional modulation of human motion sensitivity, Vision Research, 38, 2863-2867.

18.

Reynolds, J. H., Pasternak, T., Desimone, R. (2000). Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron. 26, 703-714.

19.

Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (1997). Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science, 278, 1616-1619.

20.

Snowden, R. J. (1989). Motions in orthogonal directions are mutually suppressive. Journal of Optical Society of America A, 6, 7, 1096-1101.

21.

Snowden, R. J., Treue, S., Erickson, R. G., & Anderson, R. (1991). The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion. The journal of Neuroscience, 11, 2768-2785.

22.

Snowden, R. J. & Verstraten, F. A. (1999). Motion transparency: making models of motion perception transparent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 10,369-377.

23.

Sohn, W., Papathomas, T. V., Blaser, E., & Vidnyánszky. Z. (2004). Object-based cross-feature attentional modulation from color to motion. Vision Research 44, 1437-1443.

24.

Treisman, A. & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology. 12, 97-136.

25.

Treue, S., Hol, K., & Rauber, H. J. (2000). Seeing multiple directions of motion – physiology and psychophysics. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 270-276.

26.

Treue, S. & Maunsell, J. H. (1996). Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST. Nature, 382, 539-541.

27.

Treue, S. & Maunsell, J. H. (1999). Effects of attention on the processing of motion in macaque middle temporal and medial superior temporal visual cortical areas. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 7591-7602.

28.

Valdés-Sosa, M., Cobo, A., & Pinilla, T. (1998). Transparent motion and object-based attention. Cognition 66, B13-23.

29.

Valdés-Sosa, M., Cobo, A., & Pinilla, T. (2000). Attention to object files defined by transparent motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 2, 488-505.

30.

Wade, N. J. & Verstraten, F. A. J. (1998). Introduction and historical overview. In Mather, G., Verstraten, F. A. J., & Anstis, S. (Eds.). The motion aftereffect: a modern perspective. The MIT Press Cambridge, Massachusetts.

31.

Wanning. A., Rodríguez, V. & Freiwald, W. A. (2007). Attention to surfaces modulates motion processing in extrastriate area MT. Neuron 54, 639-651.

32.

Watamaniuk, S. N. J., Sekuler, R. Williams, D. W. (1989). Direction perception in complex dynamic displays – the integration of direction information. Vision Research, 29, 47-59.

33.

Wolgemuth, A. (1911). On the after-effect of seen movement. British Journal of Psychology (Supp). 1, 1-117.

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물