바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

무의식적 내인성 주의

Unconscious Endogenous Attention

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물 / The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, (P)1226-9654; (E)2733-466X
2017, v.29 no.1, pp.21-40
https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2017.29.1.002
지은희 (연세대학교)
김민식 (연세대학교)

초록

의식하 수준으로 제시된 중심 단서의 예측력은 내인성 주의 유도에 영향을 주는가? 본 연구는 시각 단서 과제에서 중심 단서 유무에 대한 하향적 사전 지식을 갖지 못하도록 통제한 뒤, 위의 질문에 대답하려 하였다. Posner(1980)의 중심 단서 패러다임을 사용하여 내인성 주의를 조작하였으며, 중심 단서를 의식하(subliminal) 수준으로 제시하기 위하여 연속영상인식억제(CFS, Continuous Flash Suppression, Tsuchiya & Koch, 2005) 기법을 사용하였다. 실험 1에서는 예측력이 높은 중심 단서를 의식하로 제시하여 내인성 단서 효과를 측정하였다. 실험 결과, 참가자들은 중심 단서의 제시 유무를 전혀 알지 못했음에도 불구하고 단서 효과를 보였다. 즉, 의식적으로 지각할 수 없음에도 불구하고, 중심 단서가 표적을 가리키는 조건에서의 반응 시간이 그렇지 않은 조건에 비해 통계적으로 유의미하게 빠른 것으로 나타났다. 실험 2에서는 예측력이 없는 중심 단서를 의식하 수준으로 제시한 집단과 의식상(supraliminal) 수준으로 제시한 집단으로 나누어 내인성 단서 효과를 측정하였다. 실험 결과, 의식상 조건에서는 단서 효과가 관찰된 반면, 의식하 조건에서는 단서 효과가 관찰되지 않았다. 이러한 결과는 무의식적으로도 내인성 주의가 작동할 수 있으며, 무의식적 주의 통제가 단서의 예측력에 따라 다른 방식으로 작동될 수 있음을 시사한다.

keywords
unconsciousness, endogenous attention, subliminal cue, CFS, 무의식, 내인성 주의, 의식하 단서, CFS

Abstract

The current study investigated whether the predictability of the central cue would influence endogenous attention without being perceived and aware of. We used Posner’s cueing paradigm (1980) with central cues, which were subliminally presented through CFS(Continuous Flash Suppression, Tsuchiya & Koch, 2005). In Experiment 1, subliminal predictive central cue was presented and participants were not told about the existence of the central cue. Results showed that the target presented on the cued location was detected more quickly with compared to the target presented on the un-cued location, even thought they were not awared of the central cues. In Experiment 2, non-predictive cue was presented subliminally or supraliminally depending on the group condition. A random half of the participants were in the subliminal cue group, or consciously not perceived the cue. The remainder was assigned to supraliminal cue group, and they could consciously perceived the cue. Results showed that the cueing effect of the non-predictive cue was not observed in the subliminal cue group, whereas it was only reported in the supraliminal cue group. It could be inferred from these results that endogenous attention could work without conscious awareness, and it could be determined by the predictability of the subliminal cue.

keywords
unconsciousness, endogenous attention, subliminal cue, CFS, 무의식, 내인성 주의, 의식하 단서, CFS

참고문헌

1.

Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2006). Shifts of visuospatial attention to invisible (metacontrastmasked)singletons: Clues from reaction times and event-related potentials. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, 61-76.

2.

Ansorge, U., Kunde, W., & Kiefer, M. (2014). Unconscious vision and executive control: How unconscious processing and conscious action control interact. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 268-287.

3.

Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2005). Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrastmasked primes: evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cuing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(4), 762.

4.

Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2003). How conscious experience and working memory interact. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 166-172.

5.

Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2007). An architectural model of conscious and unconscious brain functions: Global Workspace Theory and IDA. Neural Networks, 20, 955-961.

6.

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556-559.

7.

Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47-89.

8.

Breitmeyer, B. G. (2007). Visual masking: past accomplishments, present status, future developments. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 9-20.

9.

Breitmeyer, B., & Öğmen, H. (2006). Visual masking: Time slices through conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

10.

Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42-45.

11.

Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79, 1-37.

12.

Duncan, J. (1998). Converging levels of analysis in the cognitive neuroscience of visual attention. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 353, 1307-1317.

13.

Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 269-297.

14.

Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030.

15.

Friesen, C. K., Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 319-329.

16.

Gayet, S., Van der Stigchel, S., & Paffen, C. L. (2014). Seeing is believing: Utilization of subliminal symbols requires a visible relevant context. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 489-507.

17.

Hassin, R. R. (2013). Yes It Can: On the Functional Abilities of the Human Unconscious. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 195- 207.

18.

Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., Engell, A. D., & McCulloch, K. C. (2009). Implicit working memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 665-678.

19.

Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 787-795.

20.

Hommel, B., Pratt, J., Colzato, L., & Godijn, R. (2001). Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychological Science, 12, 360-365.

21.

Hsieh, P. J., & Colas, J. T. (2012). Awareness is necessary for extracting patterns in working memory but not for directing spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1085-1090.

22.

Ivanoff, J., & Klein, R. M. (2003). Orienting of attention without awareness is affected by measurement-induced attentional control settings. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 3, 32-40.

23.

Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement. In J. B. Long, Baddeley, A. D. (Ed.), Attention and Performance IX (pp. 187-203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

24.

Kiefer, M. (2012). Executive control over unconscious cognition: attentional sensitization of unconscious information processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.

25.

Kim, C. Y., & Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible 'invisible'. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 381-388.

26.

Kim, M., & Lee, Y. A. (2011). The Effect of Unconscious Thought on Goal-Dependent Decision Making. Korean Journal of Cognitive Science, 22, 405-427.

27.

Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 857-875.

28.

Kunde, W. (2003). Sequential modulations of stimulus-response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 198-205.

29.

Kwon, M. Y., & Kim, M-S. (2004). The Influence of Executive Working Memory on Exogenous and Endogenous Cuing Effects. Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, 16, 451-466.

30.

Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 571-579.

31.

Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L., & Hubel, D. H. (2004). The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 229-240.

32.

Mayr, U. (2004). Conflict, consciousness, and control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 145-148.

33.

McCormick, P. A. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 168-180.

34.

Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.

35.

Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

36.

Moran, J., & Desimone, R. (1985). Selective Attention Gates Visual Processing in the Extrastriate Cortex. Science, 229, 782-784.

37.

Mulckhuyse, M., Talsma, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2007). Grabbing attention without knowing:Automatic capture of attention by subliminal spatial cues. Visual Cognition, 15, 779-788.

38.

Mulckhuyse, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. Acta Psychologica, 134, 299-309.

39.

Nummenmaa, L., & Hietanen, J. K. (2006). Gaze distractors influence saccadic curvature:Evidence for the role of the oculomotor system in gaze-cued orienting. Vision Research, 46, 3674-3680.

40.

Palmer, S., & Mattler, U. (2013). Masked stimuli modulate endogenous shifts of spatial attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 486-503.

41.

Park, H., Byoun, S., & Kwak, H.-W. (2016). The Effect of Invisible Cue on Change Detection Performance : using Continuous Flash Suppression. Korean Journal of Cognitive Science, 27, 1-25.

42.

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of Attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25.

43.

Posner, M. I., Cohen, Y., & Rafal, R. D. (1982). Neural systems control of spatial orienting. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 298, 187-198.

44.

Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information Processing and Cognition:The Loyola Symposium (pp.55-85). Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum.

45.

Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 109, 160-174.

46.

Pratt, J., Radulescu, P., Guo, R. M., & Hommel, B. (2010). Visuospatial attention is guided by both the symbolic value and the spatial proximity of selected arrows. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1321.

47.

Rafal, R. D., Calabresi, P. A., Brennan, C. W., & Sciolto, T. K. (1989). Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 15, 673.

48.

Reuss, H., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., & Wuhr, P. (2012). A cue from the unconscious - masked symbols prompt spatial anticipation. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 397.

49.

Reuss, H., Pohl, C., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2011). Follow the sign! Top-down contingent attentional capture of masked arrow cues. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 82-91.

50.

Rolfs, M. (2009). Microsaccades: small steps on a long way. Vision Research, 49, 2415-2441.

51.

Scharlau, I., & Ansorge, U. (2003). Direct parameter specification of an attention shift:Evidence from perceptual latency priming. Vision Research, 43, 1351-1363.

52.

Sklar, A. Y., Levy, N., Goldstein, A., Mandel, R., Maril, A., & Hassin, R. R. (2012). Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 19614-19619.

53.

Taylor, T. L., & Klein, R. M. (1998). On the causes and effects of inhibition of return. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 625-643.

54.

Theeuwes, J. (1994). Endogenous and exogenous control of visual selection. Perception, 23, 429-440.

55.

Theeuwes, J. (2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 65-70.

56.

Tipples, J. (2002). Eye gaze is not unique:Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin &Review, 9, 314-318.

57.

Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1096-1101.

58.

Van der Stigchel, S. (2010). Recent advances in the study of saccade trajectory deviations. Vision Research, 50, 1619-1627.

59.

van Gaal, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. (2008). Frontal cortex mediates unconsciously triggered inhibitory control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 8053-8062.

60.

van Gaal, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. (2010). Unconscious activation of the prefrontal no-go network. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4143-4150.

61.

Watanabe, M., Cheng, K., Murayama, Y., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Tanaka, K., & Logothetis, N. (2011). Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1during binocular suppression. Science, 334, 829-831.

62.

Xu, S., Zhang, S., & Geng, H. (2011). Gaze-induced joint attention persists under high perceptual load and does not depend on awareness. Vision Research, 51, 2048-2056.

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물