ISSN : 1226-9654
The perceptual load theory (Lavie & Tsal, 1994) and the dilution account (Tsal & Benoni, 2010) have been proposed to explain the phenomenon that the degree of irrelevant information processing decreases as the relevant stimulus-set size increases. The present study investigated the nature of the set-size effect on processing of task-irrelevant information. Under high perceptual load with a single distractor, no congruency effect was replicated in the present study. However, importantly, the congruency effect increased as the number (ratio) of distractor increased (Experiments 1 & 2). In dilution condition (Experiment 3), a larger congruency effect was found when a conflict distractor was located at the task-relevant array than at a task-irrelevant peripheral position, which is consistent with previous findings. However, an additional presentation of a distracting letter did not produce a larger congruency effect. These results indicate that the perceptual load effect by increasing the number of task-relevant items is a result of a reduced probability of attentional capture by a conflicting distractor. Furthermore, this selective processing occurs at a focused attention stage which implies that early-visual crosstalk is not an alternative explanation.
Bahrami, B., Carmel, D., Walsh, V., Rees, G., & Lavie, N. (2008). Unconscious orientation processing depends on perceptual load. Journal of Vision, 8(3), 1-10.
Bahrami, B., Lavie, N., & Rees, G. (2007). Attentional load modulates responses of human primary visual cortex to invisible stimuli. Current Biology, 17(6), 509-513.
Benoni, H., & Tsal, Y. (2009). Diluting the burden of load: Perceptual load effects are simply dilution effects. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 228.
Benoni, H., & Tsal, Y. (2010). Where have we gone wrong? Perceptual load does not affect selective attention. Vision Research, 50(13), 1292-1298.
Benoni, H., Zivony, A., & Tsal, Y. (2014). Attentional sets influence perceptual load effects, but not dilution effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(4), 785-792.
Biggs, A., & Gibson, B. (2010). Competition between color salience and perceptual load during visual selection can be biased by top-down set. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(1), 53-64.
Bishop, S. J., Jenkins, R., & Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Neural processing of fearful faces: effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cerebral Cortex, 17(7), 1595-1603.
Bjork, E. L., & Murray, J. T. (1977). On the nature of input channels in visual processing. Psychological Review, 84(5), 472-484.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433-436.
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). The selective nature of learning Perception and communication (pp. 244-267). Elmsford, NY, US: Pergamon Press.
Brown, T. L., Roos-Gilbert, L., & Carr, T. H. (1995). Automaticity and word perception: Evidence from Stroop and Stroop dilution effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(6), 1395-1411.
Cartwright-Finch, U., & Lavie, N. (2007). The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102(3), 321-340.
Cosman, J., & Vecera, S. (2010). Attentional capture under high perceptual load. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(6), 815-820.
Chen, Z., & Cave, K. R. (2013). Perceptual load vs. dilution: the roles of attentional focus, stimulus category, and target predictability. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 327.
Cho, Y. S., Lien, M.-C., & Proctor, R. W. (2006). Stroop dilution depends on the nature of the color carrier but not on its location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 826-839.
Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70(1), 80-90.
Eltiti, S., Wallace, D., & Fox, E. (2005). Selective target processing: Perceptual load or distractor salience? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 67(5), 876-885.
Estes, W. (1972). Interactions of signal and background variables in visual processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 12(3), 278-286.
Estes, W. (1974). Redundancy of noise elements and signals in visual detection of letters. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 16(1), 53-60.
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2007). High perceptual load makes everybody equal. Psychological Science, 18(5), 377-381.
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(1), 73-83.
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the wandering mind: The role of perceptual load. Cognition, 111(3), 345-355.
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2011). Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1064- 1070.
Gibson, B. S., & Bryant, T. A. (2008). The identity intrusion effect: Attentional capture or perceptual load? Visual Cognition, 16(2-3), 182-199.
He, C., & Chen, A. (2010). Interference from familiar natural distractors is not eliminated by high perceptual load. Psychological Research, 74(3), 268-276.
Hoffman, J. (1979). A two-stage model of visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 25(4), 319-327.
Kahneman, D., & Chajczyk, D. (1983). Tests of the automaticity of reading: Dilution of Stroop effects by color-irrelevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(4), 497-509.
Kim, H., Cho, Y., Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. (2008). Influence of color word availability on the Stroop color-naming effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 70(8), 1540-1551.
Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451-468.
Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75-82.
Lavie, N., & Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of visual selective attention: efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8(5), 395-396.
Lavie, N., & Fox, E. (2000). The role of perceptual load in negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1038-1052.
Lavie, N., & Torralbo, A. (2010). Dilution: A theoretical burden or just load? A reply to Tsal and Benoni (2010). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(6), 1657-1664.
Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 56(2), 183-197.
Macleod, C., & Hodder, S. L. (1998). Presenting two incongruent color words on a single Stroop trial does not alter Stroop interference. Memory & Cognition, 26(2), 212-219.
Marciano, H., & Yeshurun, Y. (2011). The effects of perceptual load in central and peripheral regions of the visual field. Visual Cognition, 19(3), 367-391.
Miller, J. (1982). Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals. Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 247-279.
Mitchell, D. G. V., Nakic, M., Fridberg, D., Kamel, N., Pine, D. S., & Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The impact of processing load on emotion. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1299-1309.
Muggleton, N., Lamb, R., Walsh, V., & Lavie, N. (2008). Perceptual load modulates visual cortex excitability to magnetic stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100(1), 516-519.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. East Norwalk, CT, US: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437-442.
Roper, Z. J. J., Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2013). Perceptual load corresponds with factors known to influence visual search. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 39(5), 1340-1351.
Sand, A., & Wiens, S. (2011). Processing of unattended, simple negative pictures resists perceptual load. NeuroReport, 22(7), 348-352.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Gardner, G. T. (1972). Visual processing capacity and attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93(1), 72-82.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Geisler, W. S. (1973). Visual recognition in a theory of information processing Contemporary issues in cognitive psychology: The Loyola Symposium (pp. xi, 348). Oxford, England: V. H. Winston & Sons.
Suh, J., & Cho, Y. (2013). The effect of the chance of a distractor capturing attention on distractor interference. The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, 25, 359-382.
Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A., & Belopolsky, A. (2004). Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(4), 697-702.
Tsal, Y., & Benoni, H. (2010a). Diluting the burden of load: Perceptual load effects are simply dilution effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(6), 1645-1656.
Tsal, Y., & Benoni, H. (2010b). Much dilution little load in Lavie and Torralbo's (2010) response: A reply. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(6), 1665-1668.
Wilson, D. E., Muroi, M., & MacLeod, C. M. (2011). Dilution, not load, affects distractor processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 319-335.
Yantis, S. (1993). Stimulus-driven attentional capture. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2(5), 156-161.