바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

자극의 정서와 개인의 정서 상태가 거리 지각에 미치는 영향

The Effect of Emotional Valence of Stimuli and Emotional State of Observers on Distance Perception

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물 / The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, (P)1226-9654; (E)2733-466X
2017, v.29 no.4, pp.331-349
https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2017.29.4.001
선지수 (연세대학교)
양재원 (가톨릭대학교)
정상철 (연세대학교)
  • 다운로드 수
  • 조회수

초록

인간에게 다른 사람과의 3차원 상의 공간적 거리를 지각하고 표상하는 일은 사회적 상호작용 측면에서 중요한 정보를 제공한다. 이때, 상호작용의 대상은 그 자체로 정서가를 표현할 수 있으며, 지각하는 개인 또한 다양한 정서 변화를 경험할 수 있다. 본 연구에서는 자극의 정서가에 따라 거리 지각이 달라지는지를 알아보고자 하였다. 또한, 거리를 표상하는 개인의 현재 정서 상태가 공간적 거리의 표상에 영향을 미치는지 알아보고자 하였다. 이를 알아보기 위하여, 참가자에게 긍정 혹은 부정 정서를 유도한 후, 정서가 있는 얼굴(기쁨, 분노) 혹은 중립 자극(집)과 자신 간의 공간적 거리를 추정하는 과제를 수행하도록 하였다. 실험 결과, 참가자들은 정서가 있는 얼굴을 중립 자극보다 가까이 제시된 것으로 추정하였으며, 기쁜 얼굴을 화난 얼굴에 비해 더 가까이 제시된 것으로 추정하였다. 정서 유도 조건에 따라 참가자의 정서가 변하였음에도 불구하고, 참가자의 정서 상태는 거리 추정 과제에 영향을 미치지 않는 것으로 나타났다. 이러한 결과는 자극의 정서만이 대상에 대한 거리 지각에 영향을 미친다는 것을 시사한다.

keywords
distance perception, emotional valence of stimuli, mood induction, 거리 지각, 자극의 정서, 정서 유도

Abstract

Perceiving distance from oneself to other people plays an important role in social interaction. In this situation, interaction partners can express emotional valence and a perceiver can be in various moods. The current study investigated whether spatial distance perception varies depending on emotional valence of stimuli. We also manipulated emotional state of participants. After participants were induced to feel positive or negative, they estimated the spatial distance between themselves and emotional faces (angry or happy) or neutral images (house). The results showed that the estimated distance of the emotional faces was closer than that of the neutral images. In addition, the estimated distance of the happy faces was closer than that of the angry faces. Although emotional states of participants changed depending on the mood induction condition, participants’ emotional state did not influence distance estimation. These results suggest that only emotional valence of stimuli influences distance perception.

keywords
distance perception, emotional valence of stimuli, mood induction, 거리 지각, 자극의 정서, 정서 유도

참고문헌

1.

Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 258-281.

2.

Bae, H., Kim, K., & Yi, D.-J. (2014). Effects of familiarity on the construction of psychological distance. Korean Journal of Cognitive Science, 25, 109-133.

3.

Balcetis, E. (2016). Approach and avoidance as organizing structures for motivated distance perception. Emotion Review, 8, 115-128.

4.

Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2010). Wishful seeing more desired objects are seen as closer. Psychological Science, 21, 147-152.

5.

Bar-Anan, Y., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Algom, D. (2007). Automatic processing of psychological distance: evidence from a Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 610-622.

6.

Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076-1096.

7.

Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.

8.

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433-436.

9.

Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 308-313.

10.

Caziot, B., Valsecchi, M., Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Backus, B. T. (2015). Fast perception of binocular disparity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 909-916.

11.

Cole, S., Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2013). Affective signals of threat increase perceived proximity. Psychological Science, 24, 34-40.

12.

DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., & Rouby, D. A. (2009). Social exclusion and early-stage interpersonal perception: selective attention to signs of acceptance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 729-741.

13.

Dodgson, N. A. (2004, January). Variation and extrema of human interpupillary distance. In Proceedings of SPIE, 5291, 36-46.

14.

Dosey, M. A., & Meisels, M. (1969). Personal space and self-protection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 93-97.

15.

Durgin, F. H., Baird, J. A., Greenburg, M., Russell, R., Shaughnessy, K., & Waymouth, S. (2009). Who is being deceived? The experimental demands of wearing a backpack. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 964-969.

16.

Durgin, F. H., DeWald, D., Lechich, S., Li, Z., & Ontiveros, Z. (2011). Action and motivation:Measuring perception or strategies?. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1077-1082.

17.

Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Attention, Perception, &Psychophysics, 63, 1004-1013.

18.

Firestone, C. (2013). How “paternalistic” is spatial perception? Why wearing a heavy backpack doesn’t-and couldn’t-make hills look steeper. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 455-473.

19.

Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for “top-down” effects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 1-77.

20.

Garcia-Marques, T., Mackie, D. M., Claypool, H. M., & Garcia-Marques, L. (2004). Positivity can cue familiarity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 585-593.

21.

Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34-40.

22.

Goff, P. A., Steele, C. M., & Davies, P. G. (2008). The space between us: stereotype threat and distance in interracial contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 91-107.

23.

Greenhouse, S. W., & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24, 95-112.

24.

Harber, K. D., Yeung, D., & Iacovelli, A. (2011). Psychosocial resources, threat, and the perception of distance and height: support for the resources and perception model. Emotion, 11, 1080-1090.

25.

Hayduk, L. A. (1978). Personal space: An evaluative and orienting overview. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 117-134.

26.

Heuer, K., Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Avoidance of emotional facial expressions in social anxiety: The Approach-Avoidance Task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2990-3001.

27.

Kennedy, D. P., Gläscher, J., Tyszka, J. M., & Adolphs, R. (2009). Personal space regulation by the human amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1226-1227.

28.

Kim, H.-S. (2001). Memory bias in subtypes of social phobia. Master dissertation, Seoul National University, Seoul, Korea.

29.

Kim, K., Rosenthal, M. Z., Gwaltney, M., Jarrold, W., Hatt, N., McIntyre, N., ..., & Mundy, P. (2015). A virtual joy-stick study of emotional responses and social motivation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 3891-3899.

30.

Kim, N. G., & Son, H. (2015). How Facial Expressions of Emotion Affect Distance Perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1-5.

31.

Lander, K., & Metcalfe, S. (2007). The influence of positive and negative facial expressions on face familiarity. Memory, 15, 63-69.

32.

Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2014). Traversing psychological distance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 364-369.

33.

Maher, S., Ekstrom, T., & Chen, Y. (2014). Greater perceptual sensitivity to happy facial expression. Perception, 43, 1353-1364.

34.

Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 455-470.

35.

McKee, S. P., & Mitchison, G. J. (1988). The role of retinal correspondence in stereoscopic matching. Vision Research, 28, 1001-1012.

36.

Miles, L. K. (2009). Who is approachable?. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 262-266.

37.

Nederlof, A. F., Muris, P., & Hovens, J. E. (2014). Anger, anxiety, and feelings of delusional threat as predictors of aggressive attitudes: An experimental mood induction study in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 57, 25-30.

38.

O'Shea, R. P., Blackburn, S. G., & Ono, H. (1994). Contrast as a depth cue. Vision Research, 34, 1595-1604.

39.

Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442.

40.

Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110-122.

41.

Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14, 106-112.

42.

Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition?: The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 341-365.

43.

Riener, C. R., Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Clore, G. (2011). An effect of mood on the perception of geographical slant. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 174-182.

44.

Ruggiero, G., Frassinetti, F., Coello, Y., Rapuano, M., di Cola, A. S., & Iachini, T. (2016). The effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spaces. Psychological Research, 1-9.

45.

Schnall, S., Harber, K. D., Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Social support and the perception of geographical slant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1246-1255.

46.

Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being:Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513-523.

47.

Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., Clore, G. L., & Parekh, N. (2008). Skating down a steeper slope: Fear influences the perception of geographical slant. Perception, 37, 321-323.

48.

Stefanucci, J. K., & Storbeck, J. (2009). Don't look down: emotional arousal elevates height perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 138, 131-145.

49.

Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64, 153-181.

50.

Stevens, S. S. (1961). To honor Fechner and repeal his law. Science, 133, 80-86.

51.

Stevens, S. S. (1962). The surprising simplicity of sensory metrics. American Psychologist, 17, 29-39.

52.

Stins, J. F., Roelofs, K., Villan, J., Kooijman, K., Hagenaars, M. A., & Beek, P. J. (2011). Walk to me when I smile, step back when I’m angry: emotional faces modulate whole-body approach-avoidance behaviors. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 603-611.

53.

Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. (2001). Emotional facial expressions capture attention. Neurology, 56, 153-158.

54.

Winton, E. C., Clark, D. M., & Edelmann, R. J. (1995). Social anxiety, fear of negative evaluation and the detection of negative emotion in others. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 193-196.

55.

Xiao, J., Hays, J., Ehinger, K. A., Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2010, June). SUN database:Large-scale scene recognition from abbey to zoo. In Computer vision and pattern recognition (CVPR), 2010 IEEE conference on (pp. 3485-3492). IEEE.

56.

Yang, J.-W., Chung, K., & Chong, S. C. (2015). The Yonsei Face Database. Institute of Cognitive Science, Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea.

57.

Yang, J.-W., & Oh, K. J. (2009). Cognitive bias and sensitivity in identification of facial emotions associated with social anxiety. Cognitive Behavior Therapy in Korea, 9, 87-107.

58.

Yeshurun, Y., & Carrasco, M. (1998). Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution. Nature, 396, 72-75.

59.

Yoon, H.-Y. (2013). The effects of positive and negative self imagery on the retrieval of autobiographical memories in social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Behavior Therapy in Korea, 13, 107-131.

60.

Yoon, K. L., Hong, S. W., Joormann, J., & Kang, P. (2009). Perception of facial expressions of emotion during binocular rivalry. Emotion, 9, 172-182.

61.

Yoon, K. L., & Zinbarg, R. E. (2007). Threat is in the eye of the beholder: Social anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguous facial expressions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 839-847.

62.

Yoon, K. L., & Zinbarg, R. E. (2008). Interpreting neutral faces as threatening is a default mode for socially anxious individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 680-685.

한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물