바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

스트레스적 경험에 대한 아동 기억의 신뢰성과 인지 및 정서적 개인차 특성들과의 관계

Explaining Variance in Children’s Recall of a Stressful Experience: Influence of Cognitive and Emotional Individual Differences

한국심리학회지 : 문화 및 사회문제 / Korean Psychological Journal of Culture and Social Issues, (P)1229-0661; (E)1229-0661
2013, v.19 no.3, pp.343-365
이승진 (서울대학교)

Abstract

This study examined the effects of various individual differences on children’s memory of a stressful experience. The participants for the current study were children (N=85) aged 4-9 years those who experienced a naturally occurring stressful experience from a dental procedure. There was overall negative relation between the level of stress and children’s memory performance. However, more interestingly, the results of this study provided some further evidence that several cognitive (i.e., receptive language ability and working memory capacity) and emotional (i.e., children's general anxiety condition, children’s self-report of pain and anxiety about the event) individual difference factors were associated with variations in children’s remembering across ages. The results suggest that the relation between stress and children’s memory might be impacted in part by children’s various individual characteristics. Furthermore, the findings are discussed in the applied context that based on the results clinical and legal professionals can tailor interviews to best meet children’s needs and capabilities, and create developmentally and individually sensitive guidelines for interviewing children in the legal system.

keywords
아동 기억, 수용적 언어 능력, 작업 기억 역량, 스트레스, 불안, children’s remembering, receptive language ability, working memory capacity, stress, anxiety

참고문헌

1.

Aartman, I. H. A., Everdingen Van, T., Hoogstraten, J., & Schuurs, A. H. B. (1998). Self-report measurements of dental anxiety and fear in children: A critical assessment. ASDC Journal of Dentistry for Children, 65, 252-258.

2.

Ackil, J. A., & Zaragoza, M. (1995). Developmental differences in witness suggestibility and memory for source. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 57-83.

3.

Arntz, A., van Eck, M., & Heijmans, M. (1990). Predictions of dental pain: the fear of any expected evil, is worse than the evil itself. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 29-41.

4.

Baddeley, A. (2003). “Working memory: looking back and looking forward”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829-39.

5.

Baker-Ward, L., Ornstein, P. A., Quinonez, R., Milano, M., Langley, H., Lee, S., & Morris, C. (2009). Children’s Memory for a Dental Procedure: The Impact of Stress and Coping on Remembering. Poster presented in Cognitive Developmental Society in San Antonio.

6.

Barrios, B. A., & O’Dell, S. L. (1998). Fears and Anxieties. In E. J. Mash, & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Treatment of Childhood Disorders (2nd ed., pp.249-337). New York: The Guilford Press.

7.

Bjorklund, D. F., Muir-Broaddus, J. E., & Schneider, W. (1990). The role of knowledge in the development of children’s strategies. In: D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Children’s strategies: Contemporary views of cognitive development (pp.93-128). Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

8.

Boland, A. M., Haden, C. A., & Ornstein, P. A. (2003). Boosting children’s memory by training mothers in the use of an elaborative conversational style as an event unfolds. Journal of cognition and development, 4, 39-65.

9.

Brown, R. T. (1989). Creativity. What are we to measure? In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.). Handbook of creativity (pp.3-32). New York: Plenum.

10.

Bruck, M., & Melnyk, L. (2004). Individual Differences in Children’s Suggestibility: A Review and Synthesis, Journal of Applied Psychology, 18, 2-48.

11.

Burgwyn-Bailes, E., Baker-Ward, L., Gordon, B. N., & Ornstein, P. A. (2001). Children’s memory for emergency medical treatment after one year: The impact of individual difference variables on recall and suggestibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, S25-S48.

12.

Calculator, S. N., & Singer, K. M. (1992). Letter to the editor: Preliminary validation of facilitated communication. Topics in Language Disorders, 12, pp.ix-xvi.

13.

Case, R. (1995). Capacity-based explanations of working memory growth: A brief history and reevaluation. In F. M. Weinert, & W. Schneider (Eds.), Memory performance and competencies: Issues in growth and development (pp.23-43). Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

14.

Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. San Diego: Academic Press.

15.

Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). Suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 403-439.

16.

Chae, Y., & Ceci, S. J. (2005). Individual differences in children’s recall and suggestibility: The effect of intelligence, temperament, and self-perceptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 383-407.

17.

Chen, E., Zeltzer, L. K., Craske, M. G., & Katz, E. R. (1999). Alteration of memory in the reduction of children’s distress during repeated aversive medical procedures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 481-490.

18.

Cho, S. C., & Choi, J. S. (1989). Development of the Korean form of the State-Trait anxiety Inventory for Children. Seoul Journal of Psychiatry, 14, 150-157.

19.

Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Malloy, L. C., & Allhusen, V. D. (2004). Verbal ability, self-control, and close relationships with parents protect children against misleading questions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1037-1058.

20.

Crossman, A. M. (2001). Predicting suggestibility: The role of individual differences and socialization. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Cornell University, NY.

21.

Cunningham, A., & Hurley, P. (2007). “A Full and Candid Account” Using Special Accommodations and Testimonial Aids to Facilitate the Testimony of Children. Overview of Issues Related to Child Testimony. http://www.lfcc.on.ca/1_Overview. pdf.

22.

Danielsdottir, G., Sigurgeirsdottir, S., Einarsdottir, H. R., & Haraldson, E. (1993). Interrogative suggestibility in children and its relationship with memory and vocabulary. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 499-502.

23.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN.

24.

Edelstein, R. S. (2006). Attachment and emotional memory: Investigating the source and extent of avoidant memory deficits. Emotion, 6, 340-345.

25.

Eisen, M. L., Goodman, G., Qin, J., & Davis, S. L. (2002). Memory and Suggestibility in maltreated children: Age, stress arousal, dissociation, and psychopathology. Journal of experimental child psychology, 83, 167-212.

26.

Feagans, L. V., Fendt, K., & Farran, D. C. (1995). The effects of day care intervention on teacher's ratings of the elementary school discourse skills in disadvantaged children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18(2), 243-261.

27.

Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Batterman-Faunce, J. L., Riddlesberger, M., & Kuhn, G. (1997). Children’s reactions to and memory for a stressful event: Influences of age, anatomical dolls, knowledge, and parental attachment. Applied Developmental Science, 1, 54-75.

28.

Gordon, B. N., & Follmer, A. (1994). Developmental issues in judging the credibility of children’s testimony. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 283-294.

29.

Gordon, B. N., Baker-Ward, L., & Ornstein, P. A. (2001). Children’s testimony: A review of research on memory for past experiences. Clinical Child and Family Psychology, 4, 157-181.

30.

Greenhoot, A. F., Ornstein, P. A., Gordon, B. N., & Baker-ward, L. (1999). Acting out the detail of a pediatric check-up: The impact of interview condition and behavioral style on children’s memory reports. Child Development, 70(2), 363-380.

31.

Henry, L. A., & Gudjonsson, G. H. (2004). The effects of memory trace strength on eyewitness recall in children with and without intellectual disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 53-71.

32.

Hitch, G. J., & Towse, J. (1995). Working memory: What develops? In W. Schneider & F. E. Weinert (Eds.), Memory performance and competencies: Issues in growth and development (pp.3-22). Northvale, NJ: Erlbaum.

33.

Huq, A. H., Lindsay, S. J., & Roberts, J. F. (1992). Children’s expectations and recollections of discomfort associated with dental treatment. International Journal of Pediatric Dentistry, 2(1), 11-16.

34.

Imhoff, M. C., & Baker-Ward, L. (1999). Preschoolers’ suggestibility: Effects of developmentally appropriate language and interviewer supportiveness. Applied Developmental Psychology, 20, 407-429.

35.

Jaschinski, U., & Wentura, D. (2002). Misleading postevent information and working memory capacity: An individual differences approach to eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 223-231.

36.

Kenning, M., Merchant, A., & Tomkins, A. (1991). Research on the effects of witnessing parental battering: Clinical and legal policy implications. In M. Steinman (Ed.), Woman battering: Policy responses (pp.237-261). Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.

37.

Kim, Y. T., Jang, H. S., Im, S. S., & Pack, H. J. (1995). Standardized Korean version of Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Seoul Community Rehabilitation Center, South Korea.

38.

Lander, J., Hodgins, M., & Fowler-Kerry, S. (1992). Children’s pain predictions and memories. Behavior Research and Therapy, 30, 117-124.

39.

Marsac, M. L. (2008). Relationships among Psychological Functioning, Dental Anxiety, Pain Perception, and Coping in Children and Adolescents. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, The University of Toledo.

40.

McCarthy, D. (1972). McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities. New York: Psychological Corporation.

41.

McFarlane, F., Powell, M. B., & Dudgeon, P. (2002). An examination of the degree to which IQ, memory performance, socio- economic status and gender predict young children’s suggestibility. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 7, 227-239.

42.

McGuigan, F., & Salmon, K. (2004). The time to talk: The influence of the timing of adult-child talk on children’s event memory. Child Development, 75, 669-686.

43.

Merritt, K. A., Ornstein, P. A., & Spicker, B. (1994). Children’s memory for a salient medical procedure: Implications for testimony. Pediatrics, 94(1), 17-23.

44.

Murphy, B. C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S., & Guthrie, I. K. (1999). Consistency and change in children’s emotionality and regulation: A longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 413-444.

45.

Ornstein, P. A., & Elischberger, H. B. (2004). Studies of suggestibility: Some observations and suggestions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1129-1141.

46.

Ornstein, P. A., Baker-Ward, L., Gordon, B. N., Pelphrey, K. A., Tyler, C. S., & Gramzow, E. (2006). The influence of prior knowledge and repeated questioning on children’s long-term retention of the details of a pediatric examination. Developmental Psychology, 42, 332-344.

47.

Ornstein, P. A., Gordon, B. N., & Larus, D. M. (1992). Children's memory for a personally experienced event: Implication for testimony. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 49-60.

48.

Pérez-Edgar, K., & Fox, N. A. (2003). Individual differences in children's performance during an emotional Stroop task: A behavioral and electrophysiological study. Brain and Cognition, 52, 33-51.

49.

Peterson, C., & Warren, L. K. (2009). Injuries, emergency rooms, and children’s memory: Factors contributing to individual differences. In J. Quas, & R. Fivush (Eds.), Emotion and memory in development: Biological, cognitive, and social considerations (pp.60-85). Oxford University Press.

50.

Roberts, K. P., & Powell, M. B. (2005). Evidence of metacognitive awareness in young children who have experienced a repeated event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1019-1031.

51.

Roebers, C. M., & Schneider, W. (2001). Memory for an observed event in the presence of prior misinformation: developmental patterns in free recall and identification accuracy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 19, 507-524.

52.

Roebers, C. M., & Schneider, W. (2005). The strategic regulation of children’s memory performance and suggestibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 24-44.

53.

Schacht, K. C., & Marche, T. A. (2001). Individual differences in preschoolers’ suggestibility: Can self-efficacy or source monitoring ability predict susceptibility to reporting false memories? Poster accepted for presentation and the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, Mn.

54.

Simock, G., & Hayne, H. (2002). Breaking the barrier? Children fail to translate their preverbal memories into language. Psychological Science, 13(3), 225-230.

55.

Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children. Palo Alto, Consulting Psychological Press.

56.

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state- trait anxiety inventory (form y): Self evaluation questionnaire. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

57.

Templeton, L. M., & Wilcox, S. A. (2000). A tale of two representations: The misinformation effect and children’s developing theory of mind. Child Development, 71, 402-416.

58.

Thierry, K. L., Spence, M. J., & Memon, A. (2001). Before misinformation is encountered: Source monitoring decreases child witness suggestibility. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 1-26.

59.

Vandermass, M. O., Hess, T. M., & Baker-Ward, L. (1993). Does anxiety affect children’s reports of memory for a stressful event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 109- 127.

60.

Volpe, J. S. (1996). Effects of Domestic Violence on Children and Adolescents: An Overview. The American Academy of Experts in Traumatic Stress: Retrieved from: http:// www.aaets.org/article8.htm.

61.

Zonnevelda, L. N. L., McGrathb, P. J., Reidd, G. J., & Sorbia, M. J. (1997). Accuracy of children’s pain memories, Pain, 71(3), 297- 302.

한국심리학회지 : 문화 및 사회문제