ISSN : 1226-9654
이 연구는 자극 정서성이 전역/국지처리에 미치는 효과가 작업기억 용량(working memory capacity, WMC) 개인차에 따라 어떻게 달라지는지 밝히기 위해 수행되었다. 이 연구에서는 정서적 사진자극을 제시한 후 전역처리 또는 국지처리를 요구하는 자극을 제시하고서, 전역/국지자극 식별에 걸리는 반응시간, 그리고 전역자극과 국지자극이 일치하는 조건과 불일치하는 조건 사이의 반응시간 차이(간섭량)를 조사하였다. 전역/국지처리 자극의 밀도(고밀도/저밀도)와 자극의 정서가(부정/중립/긍정)가 참가자의 WMC(고용량/저용량)에 따라 반응시간과 간섭량에 어떤 영향을 미치는지 분석하였다. 연구 결과, WMC가 작은 사람은 WMC가 큰 사람에 비해 전반적으로 느린 반응속도를 보여주었다. 특히, WMC가 큰 사람은 국지처리의 경우 긍정자극에서 가장 큰 간섭량을 보였으며, 다음으로 부정자극과 중립자극 순이었다. 전역처리에서는 정서가에 따른 간섭량의 차이가 없었다. 반면, WMC가 작은 사람은 전역처리와 국지처리에 관계없이 중립자극이나 긍정자극에 비해 부정자극에서 가장 큰 간섭량을 보였다. 이 결과는 WMC가 큰 사람의 경우 전역처리 우세성(국지처리에서 전역형태에 의한 간섭)을 보이며, 긍정정서에서 전역처리 우세성이 가장 현저함을 보여준다. 반면, WMC가 작은 사람의 경우 중립자극이나 긍정자극에 비해 부정자극이 제시된 경우에 방해자극에 의한 억제가 잘 이루어지지 않았음을 시사한다.
The aim of this study was to investigate how emotional valence effect on global/local processing was influenced by individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC). Each trial of global/local visual processing task consisted of an emotional stimulus (positive/neutral/negative) followed by an hierarchical global/local stimulus. The reaction time data showed that participants with high WMC were overall faster than those with low WMC. The analysis of interference effect in high WMC demonstrated that the interference by an irrelevant global-level stimulus on local processing was larger under positive stimuli than under negative or neutral stimuli. But, in high WMC, the interference by local-level stimuli on global processing showed no emotional valence effect. On the other hand, low WMC showed larger interference effect under negative stimuli than under neutral or positive stimuli on both global and local processing. The results suggest that high WMC showed global dominance especially under positive stimuli, but low WMC showed more difficulty in controlling interference under negative stimuli than positive or neutral stimuli.
박선희, 박태진 (2011). 전역/국지처리 과제에서 정서자극이 시각적 주의범위에 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 23, 139-151.
박선희, 박태진 (2012). 유도된 기분의 정서가와 각성수준이 전역/국지처리에 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 24, 1-18.
박태진, 박선희 (2009). IAPS 자극에 대한 한국 대학생의 정서 평가. 인지과학, 20, 183 -195.
Ahmed, L., & de Fockert, J. W. (2012). Working memory load can both improve and impair selective attention: Evidence from the Navon paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1397-1405.
Armirkhiabani, G., & Lovegrove, W. J. (1999). Do the global advantage and interference effects covary? Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1308 -1319.
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224-237.
Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553-573.
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Positive affect and flexibility: Overcoming the precedence of global over local processing of visual information. Motivation and Emotion, 2, 123 -134.
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail Working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science, 16, 101-105.
Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (1994). Working memory and retrieval: A resource-dependent inhibition model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 354-373.
Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 331-335.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450-466.
Darke, S. (1988). Anxiety and working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 145-154.
Elliman, N. A., Green, M. W., Rogers, P. J., & Finch, G. M. (1997). Processing efficiency theory and the working memory system: Impairments associated with subclinical anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 31-35.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigna, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 313-332.
Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34-40.
Gay, C. E. (2008). The implications of attention control on working memory span. Georgia Institute of Technology. http://hdl.handle. net/1853/21830.
Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Tang, J., Mehmedbegouic, F., Farrow, M., Bradshaw, J., & Sheppard, D. (2006). Age-related differences in cognitive function using a global local hierarchical paradigm. Brain Research, 1124, 86-95.
Grice, G. R., Canham, L., & Boroughs, J. M. (1983). Forest before trees? It depends where you look. Perception and Psychophysics, 33, 121-128.
Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247-278.
Hofmann, W., Gschwender, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 962-977.
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: the science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199-225.
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 46-70.
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169-183.
Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory, Emotion, 3, 378-393.
Kinchla, R. A., Solis-Macias, V., & Hoffman, J. (1983). Attending to different levels of structure in a visual image. Perception and Psychophysics, 33, 1-10.
Kinchla, R. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing: “Top down”, “bottom up” or “middle-out”. Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 225-231.
Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001). The relationship of life event stress and working memory capacity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 565-579.
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity? Intelligence, 14, 389-433.
Lamb, M. R., & Robertson, L. C. (1989). The processing of hierarchical stimuli: Effects of retinal locus, locational uncertainty, and stimulus identity. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 172-181.
Lang, P. J., Bradley. M. M., & Cuthbert. B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System(IAPS): Affective ratings of Pictures and instruction manual. (Technical Report A-6). University of Florida, Gainesville. FL.
Martin, M. (1979). Local and global processing: The role of sparsity. Memory & Cognition, 7, 476-484.
Montoro, P. R., & Luna, D. (2009). Deconfounding the effects of local element spatial heterogeneity and sparsity on processing dominance. The Journal of General Psychology, 136, 407-427.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.
Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and tourette syndrome: An information processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1015-1032.
Pomerantz, J. R. (1983). Global and local precedence: Selective attention in form and motion perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 511-535.
Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2006). Working memory capacity and attention network test performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology 20, 713-721.
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403-428.
Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 440-452.
Schmeichel, B. J., & Demaree, H. A. (2010). Working memory capacity and spontaneous emotion regulation: High capacity predicts self-enhancement in response to negative feedback. Emotion, 10, 739-744.
Schmeichel, B. J., Volokhov, R. N., & Demaree H. A. (2008). Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1526-1540.
Sobel, K. V., Gerrie, M. P., Poole, B. J., & Kane, M. J. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and visual search: The roles of top-down and bottom-up processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 840-845.
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory capacity in hot and cold cognition. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. Hecker, & D. N. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology(pp.19-43). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Unsworth, N., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Working memory capacity and the antisaccade task: Individual differences in voluntary saccade control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 1302-1321.
Weissman, D. H., Mangun, G. R., & Woldorff, M. G. (2002). A role for top-down attentional orienting during interference between global and local aspects of hierarchical stimuli. NeuroImage, 17, 1266-1276.