바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

Impact of social relationships on self-related information processing and emotional experiences

Korean Psychological Journal of Culture and Social Issues / Korean Psychological Journal of Culture and Social Issues, (P)1229-0661; (E)1229-0661
2018, v.24 no.1, pp.29-47
https://doi.org/10.20406/kjcs.2018.2.24.1.29


  • Downloaded
  • Viewed

Abstract

Do social situations have an impact on an individual’s information processing and emotional experiences? Two studies were conducted to investigate relationships between self-reference effects, emotional experiences and social information processing. Study 1 examined whether biases favoring self-related stimuli could occur automatically. Participants had to judge whether sequential geometric shape–label pairs matched or mismatched. The results showed that self-related stimuli are more rapidly processed than friends/others-related stimuli. In Study 2, the participants had to recall items which were presented with different instructions (either chosen by a friend or by the computer). Here we explored whether the self-reference effect is reduced in a social learning condition. When comparing the social learning condition (seated in pairs) with the nonsocial learning condition (seated alone), the participants recalled more self-related words in the nonsocial learning condition than in the social learning condition. Importantly, the automatic self-reference effect disappeared in the social learning condition. More friends-related words were recalled in the social condition than self-related words. In addition, while tasting chocolates, the participants judged them to be more likeable in the social condition than in the nonsocial condition. These results implicated that social processing can be useful for reducing the automatic self-reference effects and shared experiences are perceived more intensely than unshared experiences.

keywords
social processing, self, self-reference effect, shared experiences, 사회적 정보처리, 자기, 자기참조효과, 경험의 공유

Reference

1.

경향신문 (2016. 11. 28). 이유있는 변신: 타깃은 혼밥, 혼술족. http://biz.khan.co.kr/khan_ art_view.html?artid=201611281134021&code =920100.

2.

권효원, 김선경, 이혜원 (2006). 한글단어재인에서 단어빈도와 의미점화효과의 관계. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 18, 203- 220.

3.

김선미, 고하영, 박수현, 양은주 (2012). 메타분석을 통한 국내 아동․청소년의 우울 관련 변인 탐색. 한국심리학회지: 문화 및 사회문제, 18, 533-555.

4.

남순현 (2008). 청소년 모방자살의 심리적 기제에 관한 탐색. 인문사회과학연구, 19, 51-87

5.

박태진 (2003). 한국어 단어의 주관적 빈도 추정치 및 단어 재인에 미치는 빈도 효과. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 15, 349- 366.

6.

임소은, 박혜경 (2015). 문화성향에 따른 거절 반응과 예측의 차이. 한국심리학회지: 문화 및사회문제, 21, 279-297.

7.

조긍호, 김은진 (2001). 문화성향과 동조행동. 한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 15, 139- 165.

8.

조긍호, 김지연, 최경순 (2009). 문화성향과 분노통제: 분노 수준과 공감의 매개효과를 중심으로. 한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 23, 69-90.

9.

조선일보(2016. 2. 18). 506만 가구가 나홀로 살아요. http://news.chosun.com/site/data/html_ dir/2016/02/18/2016021800300.html.

10.

조증열 (2001). 범주화 과제에서 한글단어의 빈도효과. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 13, 113-131.

11.

한겨레신문(2017. 8. 19). 그의 이주와, 노동과, 이산으로 지탱되는 ‘우리’. http://www.hani. co.kr/arti/society/labor/807445.html.

12.

Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience-cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 17058-17063.

13.

Allan, K., Midjord, J. P., Martin, D., & Gabbert, F. (2012). Memory conformity and the perceived accuracy of self versus other. Memory & Cognition, 40, 280-286.

14.

Banaji, M. R. & Prentice, D. A. (1994). The self in social context. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 297-332.

15.

Basden, B. H., Reysen, M. B., & Basden, D. R. (2002). Transmitting false memories in social groups. American Journal of Psychology, 115, 211-231.

16.

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529.

17.

Bellezza, E. S. (1984). The self as a mnemonic device: The role of internal cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 506-516.

18.

Boothy, E. J., Clark, M. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2014). Shared experiences are amplified. Psychological Science, 25, 2209-2216.

19.

Bower, H., & Gilligan, S. G. (1979). Remembering information related to one’s self. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 420-432.

20.

Brewer, M. (1991). The social self: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 475-482.

21.

Burger, J. M., Messian, N., Patel, S., del Prado, A., & Anderson, C. (2004). What a coincidence! The effects of incidental similarity on compliance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 35-43.

22.

Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and Aging, 21, 140-151.

23.

Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Wright, D. B. (2006). Memory conformity: Disentangling the steps toward influence during a discussion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 480-485.

24.

Hoffman, H. G., Granhag, P. A., See, S. T. K., & Loftus, E. F. (2001). Social influences on reality-monitoring decisions. Memory & Cognition, 29, 394-404.

25.

Kesebir, S., & Oishi, S. (2010). A spontaneous self-reference effect in memory: Why some birthdays are harder to remember than others. Psychological Science, 21, 1525-1531.

26.

Kuiper, N. A. (1982). Processing personal information about well-known others and the self: The use of efficient cognitive schemata. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 14, 1-12.

27.

Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Stephan, E. (2007). Psychological distance. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. New York: Guilford Press.

28.

Looser, C. E., & Wheatley, T. (2010). The tipping point of animacy: How, when, and where we perceive life in a face. Psychological Science, 21, 1854-1862.

29.

Miller, D. T., Downs, J. S., & Prentice, D. A. (1998). Minimal conditions for the creation of a unit relationship: The social bond between birthday mates. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 475-481.

30.

Nairine, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2007). Adaptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 33, 263-273.

31.

Nairine, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., Gregory, K. J., & Van Arsdall, J. E. (2009). Adaptive memory: Fitness relevance and the hunter-gatherer mind. Psychological Science, 20, 740-746.

32.

Powers, K. E., Worsham, A. L., Freeman, J. B., Wheatley, T., & Heatherton, T. F. (2014). Social connection modulates perceptions of animacy. Psychological Science, 25, 1943-1948.

33.

Rajaram, S., & Pereira-Pasarin, L. P. (2007). Collaboration can improve individual recognition memory: Evidence from immediate and delayed tests. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 95-100.

34.

Reysen, M. B. (2007). The effects of social pressure on false memories. Memory & Cognition, 35, 59-65.

35.

Reysen, M. B., & Adair, S. A. (2008). Social processing improves recall performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 197-201.

36.

Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677-688.

37.

Schneider, W., Eschmann, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.

38.

Shteynberg, G., & Apfelbaum, E. P. (2013). The power of shared experience: Simultaneous observation with similar others facilitates social learning. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 738-744.

39.

Spunt, R. P., Meyer, M. L., & Lieberman, M. D. (2015). The default mode of human brain function primes the intentional stance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1116-1124.

40.

Sui, J., He, X., & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). Perceptual effects of social salience: Evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1105- 1117.

41.

Sui, J., Sun, Y., Peng, K., & Humphreys, G. W. (2014). The automatic and the expected self: Separating self- and familiarity biases effects by manipulating stimulus. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 76, 1176-1184.

42.

Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371- 394.

43.

Van Overschelde, J. P., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the Batting and Montague (1969) norms. Journal of Memory & Language, 50, 289-335.

44.

Wright, D. B., & Klumpp, A. (2004). Collaborative inhibition is due to the product, not the process, of recalling in groups. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1080-1083.

Korean Psychological Journal of Culture and Social Issues