ISSN : 1229-0653
Regarding the altruistic punishment of a third party, an act committed by a person unrelated to the situation after witnessing an unfair situation. the inequity aversion model describes the experienced moral outrage when witnessing an unfair situation as a determinant of a third party's altruistic punishment, but does not address what cognitive processes lead to third-party punishment. Present study was intended to find out why moral outrage led to the decision of the altruistic punishment of a third party to approach the cognitive processing process through the theory of dual system and the ego-depletion incurred stronger punishment. For these purposes, 40 participants were randomly assigned to high and low ego-depletion conditions, manipulated ego-depletion, and then played third party punishment game, and measured the moral outrage experienced in each situation. Results of this study found that the more unfair situation participants witnessed, the more strongly they experienced moral outrage, but there was no difference between conditions. However, participants not only imposed stronger punishment on the more unfair the situation they witnessed, but also stronger punishments than high ego-depletion conditions were lower when the unfairness of the situation they witnessed was relatively high. These difference between conditions was significant even though controlling the effects of the moral outrage. These results indicate that the role of system 1 that is impulsive and emotion-based and system 2. which has adjust and self-control function on the altruistic punishment of third parties, and the reason why ego-depletion increases the third-party’s punishment is no because of the experience of increased moral outrage but because of the impaired function of system 2.
강내리 (2018. 11. 19). 마이크로닷, 부모 사기설파장…추가 입장 내놓을까. YTN. Retrieved from https://www.ytn.co.kr/_sn/0117_201811192004252775#share_btns
곽호완 (2011). 혼합 설계에서 공분산분석의 주의점: KwakStat과 SPSS를 사용하여. 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물, 23(2), 229-238. doi:10.22172/cogbio.2011.23.2.003
김건휘 (2018. 11. 23). 마이크로닷 母 ”잠적․도피 아니다, 조사 받을 것“. 머니투데이. Retrieved from http://news.mt.co.kr/mtview.php?no=2018112312293082694
김민지, 김범준 (2018). 자아고갈이 처벌판단에 미치는 영향. 한국심리학회지: 법, 9(2), 57-74.
박은아, 박민지 (2018). 누가 불매운동에 참여하는가?. 한국심리학회지: 소비자․광고, 19(1), 121-138. doi:10.21074/kjlcap.2018.19.1.121
이보영 (2014. 12. 08). “감히 땅콩을 봉지째로? 비행기 돌려” KAL 조현아 부사장 갑질논란. 뉴데일리. Retrieved from http://biz.newdaily.co.kr/site/data/html/2014/12/08/2014120810007.html
이윤재, 강명수, 이한석 (2013). 온라인 소비자 불매운동의도의 영향요인에 관한 연구: 온라인 익명성을 중심으로. 소비자문제연구, 44(2), 27-44. doi:10.15723/jcps.44.2.201308.27
장정헌, 김선호 (2014). 공감, 책임귀인, 그리고 분노가 이타적 처벌 의사에 미치는 영향:2013년 남양유업 사건을 중심으로. 한국언론학보, 58(5), 97-122.
최승혁, 허태균 (2011). 공정한 사회를 위한 형사처벌: 공정세상 믿음 및 기대의 상호작용. 한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 25(2), 113-125. doi:10.21193/kjspp.2011.25.2.006
최승혁, 허태균 (2012). 잘난 사람의 범죄는?: 처벌판단에서 사회경제적 지위의 역할과 그 심리기제. 한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 26(4), 127-140. doi:10.21193/kjspp.2012.26.4.008
홍세은, 이현정, 허태균 (2018). 처벌판단에서 행위자의 사회경제적 지위와 의도 불확실성의 역할. 한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 32 (3), 101-118. doi:10.21193/kjspp.2018.32.3.006 https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208323981
Ainsworth, S. E., Baumeister, R. F., Ariely, D., & Vohs, K. D. (2014). Ego depletion decreases trust in economic decision making. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 40-49. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2014.04.004
Barclay, P. (2006). Reputational benefits for altruistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(5), 325-344. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.01.003
Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 65, 313-319.
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2018). Strength model of self-regulation as limited resource:Assessment, controversies, update. In Baumeister, R. F (Ed.), Self-Regulation and Self-Control (pp. 78-128). Routledge. doi:10.4324/9781315175775-3
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252-1265. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1252
Baumeister, R. F., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Ego depletion: A resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing. Social cognition, 18(2), 130-150. doi:10.1521/soco.2000.18.2.130
Bendor, J., & Swistak, P. (2001). The evolution of norms. American Journal of Sociology, 106(6), 1493-1545. doi:10.1086/321298
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171-195. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(92)90032-y
Buckholtz, J. W. (2015). Social norms, self-control, and the value of antisocial behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 122-129.
Caldwell, M. D. (1976). Communication and sex effects in a five-person Prisoner's Dilemma Game. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33(3), 273-280. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.33.3.273
Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., Lieberman, M. D., Tabibnia, G., & Robbins, T. W. (2010). Impulsive choice and altruistic punishment are correlated and increase in tandem with serotonin depletion. Emotion, 10(6), 855-862. doi:10.1037/a0019861
De Kwaadsteniet, E. W., Rijkhoff, S. A. M., & van Dijk, E. (2013). Equality as a benchmark for third-party punishment and reward: The moderating role of uncertainty in social dilemmas. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 120(2), 251-259. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.06.007
De Neys, W., & Glumicic, T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition, 106(3), 1248-1299. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.002
Delton, A. W., & Krasnow, M. M. (2017). The psychology of deterrence explains why group membership matters for third-party punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(6), 734-743. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.07.003
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M. T., & Maner, J. K. (2008). Depletion Makes the Heart Grow Less Helpful: Helping as a Function of Self-Regulatory Energy and Genetic Relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(12), 165301662.
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Putting adjustment back in the anchoring and adjustment heuristic:Differential processing of self-generated and experimenter-provided anchors. Psychological Science, 12(5), 391-396. doi:10.1111/1467-9280. 00372
Epstein, S., & Pacini, R. (1999). Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 462-482). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2003). In two minds:dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(10), 454-459. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2006). The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 378-395. doi:10.3758/bf03193858
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2007). On the resolution of conflict in dual process theories of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 13(4), 321-339. doi:10.1080/13546780601008825
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 255-278. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition:Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223-241. doi:10.1177/1745691612460685
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 63-87. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980-994. doi:10.1257/aer.90.4.980
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868), 137-140. doi:10.1038/415137a
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The quarterly journal of economics, 114(3), 817-868. doi:10.1162/003355399556151
Fowler, J. H., Johnson, T., & Smirnov, O. (2005). Human behaviour: Egalitarian motive and altruistic punishment. Nature, 433(7021), E1-E2. doi:10.1038/nature03256
Galak, J., & Chow, R. M. (2019). Compensate a little, but punish a lot: Asymmetric routes to restoring justice. PLOS ONE, 14(1), e0210676. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0210676
Gintis, H. (2000). Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 206(2), 169-179. doi:10.1006/jtbi.2000.2111
Gronchi, G., & Giovannelli, F. (2018). Dual Process Theory of Thought and Default Mode Network:A Possible Neural Foundation of Fast Thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1237. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01237
Gummerum, M., Van Dillen, L. F., Van Dijk, E., & López-Pérez, B. (2016). Costly third-party interventions: The role of incidental anger and attention focus in punishment of the perpetrator and compensation of the victim. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 94-104. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2016.04.004
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 19931-19936. doi:10.1073/pnas.1110306108
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., et al. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767-1770. doi:10.1126/science. 1127333
Herrmann-Pillath, C. (2019). From dual systems to dual function: rethinking methodological foundations of behavioural economics. Economics & Philosophy, 1-20. doi:10.1017/s0266267118000378
Jensen, K. (2010). Punishment and spite, the dark side of cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 2635-2650. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0146
Jordan, J., McAuliffe, K., & Rand, D. (2016). The effects of endowment size and strategy method on third party punishment. Experimental Economics, 19(4), 741-763. doi:10.1007/s10683-015-9466-8
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Knoch, D., & Fehr, E. (2007). Resisting the power of temptations: The right prefrontal cortex and self‐control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104(1), 123-134.
Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2007). Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: evidence from the Ultimatum Game. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(4), 951-956. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4606-06.2007
Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P., & O'Brien, E. (2007). Audience effects on moralistic punishment. Evolution and Human behavior, 28(2), 75-84. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.06.001
Lieberman, D., & Linke, L. (2007). The effect of social category on third party punishment. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(2), 289-305. doi:10. 1177/147470490700500203
Liu, Y., He, N., & Dou, K. (2015). Ego-depletion promotes altruistic punishment. Open Journal of Social Sciences, 3(11), 62-69. doi:10.4236/jss.2015. 311009
Lotz, S., Baumert, A., Schlösser, T., Gresser, F., & Fetchenhauer, D. (2011). Individual differences in third‐party interventions: How justice sensitivity shapes altruistic punishment. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 4(4), 297-313. doi:10.1111/j.1750-4716.2011.00084.x
McAuliffe, K., Jordan, J. J., & Warneken, F. (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10. doi:10.1016/j.cognition. 2014.08.013
McCall, C., Steinbeis, N., Ricard, M., & Singer, T. (2014). Compassion meditators show less anger, less punishment, and more compensation of victims in response to fairness violations. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 424. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00424
Mussweiler, T., & Ockenfels, A. (2013). Similarity increases altruistic punishment in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(48), 19318-19323. doi:10.1073/pnas. 1215443110
Nelissen, R. M. A., & Zeelenberg, M. (2009). Moral emotions as determinants of third-party punishment: Anger, guilt and the functions of altruistic sanctions. Judgment and Decision making, 4(7), 543-553.
Newell, B. R., Lagnado, D. A., & Shanks, D. R. (2015). Straight choices: The psychology of decision making(2nd ed.). London: Psychology Press. doi:10.4324/9781315727080
Ohtsubo, Y., Sasaki, S., Nakanishi, D., & Igawa, J. (2018). Within-individual associations among third-party intervention strategies: Third-party helpers, but not punishers, reward generosity. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 12(2), 113-125. doi:10.1037/ebs0000107
Ostrom, E., Walker, J., & Gardner, R. (1992). Covenants with and without a sword:Self-governance is possible. American Political Science Review, 86(2), 404-417. doi:10.2307/1964229
Pedersen, E. J., McAuliffe, W. H. B., & McCullough, M. E. (2018). The unresponsive avenger: More evidence that disinterested third parties do not punish altruistically. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(4), 514-544. doi:10.1037/xge0000410
Rabellino, D., Morese, R., Ciaramidaro, A., Bara, B. G., & Bosco, F. M. (2016). Third-party punishment: altruistic and anti-social behaviours in in-group and out-group settings. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 486-495. doi:10.1080/20445911.2016.1138961
Raihani, N. J., & McAuliffe, K. (2012). Human punishment is motivated by inequity aversion, not a desire for reciprocity. Biology letters, 8(5), 802-804. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2012.0470
Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D., & Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489(7416), 427-430. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11467
Roberts, S. C., Vakirtzis, A., Kristjánsdóttir, L., & Havlíček, J. (2013). Who punishes? Personality traits predict individual variation in punitive sentiment. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(1), 186-200. doi:10.1177/147470491301100117
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 1755-1758. doi:10.1126/science.1082976
Schlosser, M. E. (in press). Dual-system theory and the role of consciousness in intentional action. In B. Feltz, M. Missal, & A. Sims (Eds.), Free Will, Causality and Neuroscience.
Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual performance and ego depletion: Role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 33-46. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.33
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 3-22. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.119.1.3
Sloman, S. A. (2014). Two systems of reasoning: An update. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories of the social mind (pp. 69-79). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto Others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.
Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational?: Studies of individual differences in reasoning. New York:Psychology Press. doi:10.4324/9781410603432
Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot's rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. doi:10.7208/chicago/9780226771199.001.0001
Steinbeis, N., & Over, H. (2017). Enhancing behavioral control increases sharing in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 310-318.
Wagner, D. D., Altman, M., Boswell, R. G., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2013). Self-regulatory depletion enhances neural responses to rewards and impairs top-down control. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2262-2271. doi:10.1177/0956797613492985
Yudkin, D. A., Rothmund, T., Twardawski, M., Thalla, N., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). Reflexive intergroup bias in third-party punishment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(11), 1448-1459. doi:10.1037/xge0000190