ISSN : 1229-0653
This study aimed to compare false memory effects of goal priming and directed forgetting to control negative stereotypical informations about elderly adults. Participants, 22 male and 34 female university students listened to a news about increasing rates of elderly citizens in Korea living alone. This was followed by a lexical decision task with stereotypical and nonstereotypical words, to investigate the activation of aging stereotypes and to devide the participants into two groups according to high or low stereotype activation. After that, the participants read articles about young and elderly people either under the condition of goal priming or under the condition of the forget instruction. Finally, a recognition test was conducted based on the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm to compare effects of these two conditions for false memory about stereotypical and nonstereotypical items between the two groups with high or low stereotype activation. The results indicated that the false recognition rates for stereotypical items were significantly higher in the high activation group with the forget instruction than in this group of the goal priming condition. However, there were no significant differences in the false recognition rates for the nonstereotypical items. These findings supported our hypothesis, that the forget instruction activates negative stereotypical information, which resulted in the memory illusion and the disruption of stereotype control. In addition, the false recognition rates under the condition of goal priming were lower than under the condition of the forget instruction, which might be interpreted as evidence for effective stereotype control. Implications of the outcome for stereotype control are discussed.
서병숙, 김수현 (1999). 대학생의 노인에 대한 이미지연구. 한국노년학, 19, 97-111.
이영숙, 박경란 (2003). 대학생이 인지하는 남, 여 노인의 고정관념 비교연구. 노인복지연구, 19, 83-109.
한정란 (2000). 대학생들의 노인에 대한 태도에 관한연구. 한국노년학, 20, 115-127.
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits asknowledge structures: Automacity ingoal-directed behavior. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 78, 53-63.
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Holland, R. W. (2007). Thenonconscious cessation of goal pursuit: Whengoals and negative affect are coactivated.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,92, 165-178.
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Veltkamp, M. (2008). Goalpriming and the affective-motivational route tononconscious goal pursuit. Social Cognition, 26,555-577.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice.Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Anaki, D., Faran, Y., Ben-Shalom, D., & Henik, A.(2005). The false memory and the mirror effects:The role of familiarity and backward associationin creating recollections. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 52, 87-102.
Araya, T., Ekehammar, Bo., & Akrami, N. (2003).Remembering things that never occurred: Theeffects of to-be-forgotten stereotypicalinformation. Experimental Psychology, 50, 27-32.
Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automaticstereotyping. Psychological Science, 7, 136-141.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996).Automacity of social behavior: direct effects oftrait construct and stereotype activation onaction. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 71, 230-244.
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A.,Barndollar, K., & Trotschel, R. (2001). Theautomated will: Nonconscious activation andpursuit of behavioral goals. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 81,1014-1027.
Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Gargano, G. J.(2003). Directed forgetting in implicit and explicitmemory tests: A comparison of methods. Journalof experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,and Cognition, 19, 603-616.
Blair, I., & Banaji, M. (1996). Automatic andcontrolled processes in stereotype priming.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70, 1142-1163.
Bloom. H. (1987). William Blake's The marriage ofHeaven and Hell. New York: Chelsea House Publishers
Chartrand, T. L., Huber, J., Shiv, B., & Tanner, R.(2008). Nonconscious goals and consumer choice.Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 189-201.
Clusters, R., & Aarts, H. (2007). Goal-discrepantsituations prime and goal-directed actions ifgoals are temporarily or chronically accessible.Personality and social psychology bulletin, 33,623-633.
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence ofparticular verbal intrusions in immediate recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17-22.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and Prejudice:Their automatic and controlled components.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,56, 5-18.
Devine, P. G., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M.,Harmon-Jones E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). Theregulation of explicit and implicit race bias: Therole of motivations to respond without prejudice.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,82, 835-848.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., vanBaaren, R. B. (2006). On making the rightchoice: The deliberation-without-attention effect.Science, 311, 1005-1007.
Dunning, D, & Sherman, D. A. (1997). Stereotypesand tacit inference. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 73, 459-471.
Ferguson, M. J. (2007). On the automatic evaluationof end-states. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 92, 596-611.
Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005).Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41,220-239.
Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2009). Motions ofthe hand expose the partial and parallelactivation of stereotypes. Psychological Science,20, 1183-1188.
Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble ofthinking: Activation and application of stereotypicbeliefs. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 60, 509-517.
Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008). Implicitmotivation to control prejudice. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 44(1), 164-172.
Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989).Gains and losses in development throughadulthood as perceived by different adult agegroups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109-121.
Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Goals as knowledgestructures. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh(Eds.), Psychology of action: Linking conditionand motivation to behavior (pp.599-619). NewYork: Guilford Press.
Lenton, A. P., Blair, I. V., & Hastie, R. (2001).Illusions of gender: Stereotypes evoke falsememories. Journal of Experimental SocialPsychology, 37, 3-14.
Liberman, N., & Förstner, J. (2000). Expression aftersuppression: A motivational explanation ofpostsuppressional rebound. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 79, 190-203.
Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V.(1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: Apeek inside the cognitive tool box. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 66, 37-47.
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., &Wheeler, V. (1996). On resisting the temptationfor simplification: Counterintentional effects ofstereotype suppression on social memory. SocialCognition, 14, 1-20.
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., &Ford, R. L. (1997). On regulation of recollection:The intentional forgetting of stereotypicalmemories. Journal of Personality and socialPsychology, 72, 709-719.
MacLeod, C. M. (1999). The item and list methodsof directed forgetting: Test differences and therole of demand characteristics. PsychonomicBulletin and Review, 6, 123-129.
Moscowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P., Wasel, W., &Schaal, B. (1999). Preconscious control ofstereotype activation through chronic egalitariangoals on attention. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 77, 167-184.
Moscowitz, G. B. (2002). Preconscious effects oftemporary goals on attention. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 38, 397-404.
Moscowitz, G. B., & Gesundheit, Y. (2009). Goalpriming. In G. B. Moscowitz, & H. Grant (Eds.),The psychology of goals (pp.203-233). New York:The Guilford Press.
Moscowitz, G. B., & Ignarri, C. (2009). Implicitvolition and stereotype control. European View ofSocial Psychology, 20, 97-145.
Moscowitz, G. B., & Li, P. (2011). Egalitarian goalstrigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form ofstereotype control. Journal of ExperimentalSocial Psychology, 47, 103-116.
Park, S. H., Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008).Implicit motivation to control prejudice moderatesthe effect of cognitive depletion on unintendeddiscrimination. Social Cognition, 26, 401-419.
Perdue, C. W., & Gurtman, M. B. (1990). Evidencefor the automaticity of ageism. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 26, 199-216.
Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995).Creating false memories: Remembering wordsnot presented in lists. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,21, 803-814.
Roediger, H. L., III, Watson, J. M., McDermott, K.B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determinefalse recall: A multiple regression analysi Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21, 803-814.
Sela, A., & Shiv, B. (2009). Unraveling priming:When does the same prime activate a goalversus a trait? Journal of Consumer Research,36, 418-433.
Shah, J. Y. (2003). Automatic for the people: Howrepresentations of significant others implicitlyaffect goal pursuit. Journal of Personality andsocial Psychology, 84, 661-681.
Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Whenopportunity knocks: Bottom-up priming of goalsby means and its effect on self-regulation.Journal of Personality and social Psychology, 84,1109-1122.
Shah, J. Y. (2005). The automatic pursuit andmanagement of goals. Current directions inpsychological science, 14, 10-13.
Steffens, M. C., & Mecklenbräuker, S. (2007). Falsememories: Phenomena, theories, and implications.Journal of Psychology, 215, 12-34.
Tse, C. S., & Neely, J. H. (2005). Assessingactivation without source monitoring in the DRMfalse memory paradigm. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 53, 532-550.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mentalcontrol. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52