바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

거짓 진술의 인지부하가 안구움직임에 미치는 영향

The effect of cognitive load during deception on eye movements

한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격 / Korean Journal of Social and Personality Psychology, (P)1229-0653;
2013, v.27 no.2, pp.37-49
https://doi.org/10.21193/kjspp.2013.27.2.003
김수진 (중앙대학교)
이장한 (중앙대학교)

초록

본 연구에서는 거짓말자의 진술조건에 따른 인지부하의 차이가 안구움직임에 미치는 영향을 알아보고자 하였다. 이를 위해 49명의 실험참가자를 거짓 집단(24명)과 진실 집단(25명)으로 나누어 모의범죄를 수행시킨 후, 두 가지 진술조건(거꾸로 말하기, 바로 말하기)에서 진술하는 동안 안구움직임을 측정하였다. 실험 결과, 거짓 집단은 진실 집단에 비해 모든 진술조건에서 도약안구운동의 지속시간이 짧았으며, 바로 말하기 진술조건에서 눈 깜빡임 빈도가 유의미하게 낮았다. 이런 결과는 거짓말 집단에서는 거짓말의 인지부하로 인해 안구움직임은 거의 고정되거나 작은 범위로 움직이고, 눈 깜빡임이 줄어든다는 것을 의미한다. 하지만 상대적으로 높은 인지부하를 경험하는 거꾸로 말하기 진술조건에서는 두 집단 간 안구움직임의 차이는 유의미하지 않았고 바로 말하기 진술조건에 비해 전반적으로 눈 깜빡임과 짧은 시선고정을 자주 보였는데, 이는 인지부하뿐만 아니라 높은 정서불안과 관련이 있어 보인다. 본 연구는 의식적으로 통제가 불가능한 안구움직임을 사용하여 거짓말 탐지를 위한 안구움직임 지표를 탐색하였으며, 바로 말하기 조건으로 거짓말을 할 때 시선의 이동거리가 짧고 눈 깜빡임이 적은 행동을 보이는 것을 확인할 수 있었다.

keywords
거짓말 탐지, 안구움직임, 아이트랙커(Eye-tracker), 거꾸로 말하기, 바로 말하기, Deception, Detecting deception cues, Eye movements, Eye-tracker, Reverse order statement, Detect deception

Abstract

This study examined the eye movements differences between the reverse order and the chronological order statement condition to find out the effective eye movement index for detecting deception. Participants (49 university students) conducted the missions in computer lab. After conduction mock crime, they were randomly allocated to the reverse order or chronological order conditions. Using eye-tracker, we measured the eye movements during statement. Lie group showed shorter saccade duration than truth group at both reverse order and chronological order statement condition. Lie group also showed less blink than truth teller in chronological order state condition. However, there was no difference of eye movements between lie and truth group in reverse order condition. Reverse order group also showed more frequent eye blink and fixation than chronological order group. It was suggested that emotional anxiety during reverse order statement would influence the eye movement. This study found that short saccade duration and less eye blink could be the effective eye movement indexes for the detect deception.

keywords
거짓말 탐지, 안구움직임, 아이트랙커(Eye-tracker), 거꾸로 말하기, 바로 말하기, Deception, Detecting deception cues, Eye movements, Eye-tracker, Reverse order statement, Detect deception

참고문헌

1.

김정택 (1978). 특성불안과 사회성과의 관계-Spielberger의 STAI를 중심으로. 고려대학교 일반대학원 석사학위 청구 논문.

2.

조은경, 이미선, 김재홍 (2005). 거짓말의 특징에 대한 신념. 한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격, 19(1), 185-198.

3.

DePaulo, B. M., LeMay, C. S., & Epstein, J. A. (1991). Effects of importance of success and expectations for success on effectiveness at deceiving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 14-24.

4.

DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74-118.

5.

DePaulo, B. M., & Kirkendol, S. E. (1989). The motivational impairment effect in the communication of deception. In J. C. Yuille (Ed.), Credibility assessment (pp.51-70). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

6.

Dionisio, D. P., Granholm, E., Hillix, W. A., & Perrine, W. F. (2001). Differentiation of deception using pupillary response as an index of cognitive processing. Psychophysioloy, 38, 205- 211.

7.

Donkelaar, P., Saaverdra, S., Woollacott, M. (2007). Multiple saccades are more automatic than single saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 3148-3151.

8.

Ekman, P. (1988). Lying and nonverbal behavior: Theoretical issues and new findings. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 163-176.

9.

Ekman, P. (1992). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics and marriage. NY: Norton.

10.

Ekman, P., O’Sullivan, M., Friesen, W.V., & Scherer, K. (1991). Face, voice, and body in detecting deceit. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 125- 135.

11.

Freeth, M., Foulsham, T., & Chapman, P. (2011). The influence of visual saliency on fixation patterns in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychologia, 49(1), 156-160.

12.

Gog, T., Kester, L., Nievelstein, F., Giesbers, B & Paas, F. (2009). Uncovering cognitive processes: different techniques that can contribute to cognitive load research and instruction. Computer in Human Behavior, 25, 325-331.

13.

Holland, M. K., & Tarlow, G. (1972). Blinking and mental load. Psychological Reports, 31, 119-127.

14.

Holland, M. K., & Tarlow, G. (1975). Blinking and thinking. Psychological Reports, 41, 403-406.

15.

In-Albon, T., Kossowsky, J., & Schneider, S. (2010). Vigilance and avoidance of threat in the eye movements of children with separation anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 225-235.

16.

Iqbal, S. T., Zheng, X. S., & Bailey, B. P. (2004). Task evoked pupillary response to mental workload in human-computer interaction. Proceedings of CHI EA'04 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems, Austria, 1477- 1480.

17.

Kohnken, G. (1989). Behavioral correlates of statement credibility: Theories, paradigms, and results. In H. Wegener, F. Losel, & J. Haisch (Eds.), Criminal behavior and the justice system (pp.271- 289). London: Springer Verlag.

18.

Leal, S., & Vrij, A. (2008). Blinking during and after lying. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32, 187-194.

19.

McCornack, S. A. (1992). Information manipulation theory. Communication Monographs, 29, 1-16.

20.

McCornack, S. A. (1997). The generation of deceptive messages: Laying the groundwork for a viable theory of interpersonal deception. In J. O. Greene (Ed.), Message production: Advances in communication theory. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

21.

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372-422.

22.

Rognin, L., Grimaud, I., Hoffman, E., & Zeghal, K. (2004, September). Assessing the impact of a new instruction on air traffic controller monitoring tasks. Paper Presented at the International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction in Aeronautics (HCI-Aero), Toulouse, France.

23.

Spence, S. A., Farrow, T. F. D., Herford, A. E., Wilkinson, I. D., Zheng, Y., & Woodruff, P. W. R. (2001). Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 12, 2848-2853.

24.

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

25.

Underwood, G., Jebbett, L., & Roberts, K. (2004). Inspecting pictures for information to verify a sentence: Eye movements in general encoding and in focused search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 165-182.

26.

Van-Orden, K. F. V., Jung, T. P., & Makeig, S. (2000). Combined eye activity measures accurately estimate changes in sustained visual task performance. Biological Psychology, 52, 221- 240.

27.

Vrij, A. (2000). Detecting lies and deceit: The psychology of lying and its implication for professional practice. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.

28.

Vrij, A. (2004). Why professionals fail to catch liars and how they can improve. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 9, 159-181.

29.

Vrij, A., Edward, K., Robert, K. P., & Bull, R. (2000). Detecting deceit via analysis of verbal and nonverbal behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 239-263.

30.

Vrij, A., Fisher, R., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2006). Detecting deception by manipulating cognitive load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 141-142.

31.

Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2003). Deception detection. In P. W. Halligan, C. Bass, & D. A. Oakley (Eds.), Malingering and illness deception (pp.348-362). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

32.

Vrij, A., Mann, S. A., Fisher, R. P., Leal, S., Milne, R., & Bull, R. (2008). Increasing cognitive load to facilitate lie detection: The benefit of recalling an event in reverse order. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 253-265.

33.

Walczyk, J. J., Roper, K. S., Seeman, E., & Humphreys, A. M. (2003). Cognitive mechanisms underlying lying to question: Response time as a cue to deception. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 755-774.

34.

Walczyk, J. J., Schwartz, J. P., Clifton, R., Adams, B., Wei, M., & Zha, P. (2005). Lying person- to-person about life events: A cognitive framework for lie detection. Personnel Psychology, 58(1), 141-170.

35.

Wallbott, H. C., & Scherer, K. R. (1991). Stress specifics: Differential effects of coping style, gender, and type of stressor on autonomic arousal, facial expression, and subjective feeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 147-156.

36.

Zeghal, K., Grimaud, I., Hoffman, E., & Rognin, L. (2002, October). Delegation of spacing tasks from controllers to flight crew: Impact of controller monitoring tasks. Paper presented at the 21st Digital Avionic Systems Conference, Irvine, CA.

37.

Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp.1-59). NY: Academic Press.

한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격