바로가기메뉴

본문 바로가기 주메뉴 바로가기

ACOMS+ 및 학술지 리포지터리 설명회

  • 한국과학기술정보연구원(KISTI) 서울분원 대회의실(별관 3층)
  • 2024년 07월 03일(수) 13:30
 

logo

  • P-ISSN1229-0653
  • KCI

노인 고정관념의 억제와 오기억: 목표점화(goal priming) 및 망각지시의 비교

Stereotype Control and Memory Illusion: A Comparison between Goal Priming and Forget Instruction

한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격 / Korean Journal of Social and Personality Psychology, (P)1229-0653;
2011, v.25 no.2, pp.17-34
https://doi.org/10.21193/kjspp.2011.25.2.002
신홍임 (연세대학교)
김민식 (연세대학교)

초록

본 연구에서는 노인에 대한 평소의 부정적인 태도나 차별행동을 생각하게 하여 부정적인 노인 고정관념을 억제하는 목표로 암묵적으로 동기화하는 목표점화조건과 부정적인 노인 고정관념을 직접적으로 망각지시하는 조건에서 노인 고정관념에 대한 오기억(false memory)에서 차이가 나타나는지를 알아보고자 하였다. 56명의 대학생을 대상으로 부정적인 고정관념을 활성화하기 위해 실험도입부에 독거노인에 대한 뉴스를 청취하게 한 후 어휘판단과제에서 부정적인 노인 고정관념의 단어에 대한 반응시간을 토대로 고정관념이 활성화된 집단과 고정관념이 활성화되지 않은 집단으로 구분하였다. 이 후 목표점화 또는 망각지시의 조건에서 참가자들에게 노인에 대한 글을 읽도록 하고, Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM)의 패러다임을 토대로 재인과제를 실시하여 학습단계에서 제시되지 않은, 부정적인 고정관념 연관문장에 대한 오재인율을 분석하였다. 실험결과에서는 고정관념의 활성화, 지시유형 및 재인과제의 문장유형 간에 상호작용이 나타났다. 고정관념이 활성화된 집단에서는 망각지시의 조건에서 부정적인 노인 고정관념에 대한 오재인율이 목표점화의 조건보다 유의미하게 높았으며, 이러한 경향은 재인과제에서 노인고정관념과 연관된 문장에서만 나타났다. 이 결과는 고정관념 억제에 대한 직접적인 망각지시가 부정적인 고정관념을 더 많이 활성화시켜 오재인율을 높이는 데 비해 목표점화는 고정관념 억제를 암묵적으로 동기화시켜 고정관념이 활성화되는 것을 억제하는 것으로 설명할 수 있다. 논의에서는 고정관념 억제에 목표점화를 유용하게 활용할 수 있는 가능성 및 본 연구의 제한점과 후속연구를 위한 시사점을 다루었다.

keywords
stereotype control, memory illusion, goal priming, forget instruction, 고정관념, 목표점화, 망각지시, 오기억

Abstract

This study aimed to compare false memory effects of goal priming and directed forgetting to control negative stereotypical informations about elderly adults. Participants, 22 male and 34 female university students listened to a news about increasing rates of elderly citizens in Korea living alone. This was followed by a lexical decision task with stereotypical and nonstereotypical words, to investigate the activation of aging stereotypes and to devide the participants into two groups according to high or low stereotype activation. After that, the participants read articles about young and elderly people either under the condition of goal priming or under the condition of the forget instruction. Finally, a recognition test was conducted based on the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm to compare effects of these two conditions for false memory about stereotypical and nonstereotypical items between the two groups with high or low stereotype activation. The results indicated that the false recognition rates for stereotypical items were significantly higher in the high activation group with the forget instruction than in this group of the goal priming condition. However, there were no significant differences in the false recognition rates for the nonstereotypical items. These findings supported our hypothesis, that the forget instruction activates negative stereotypical information, which resulted in the memory illusion and the disruption of stereotype control. In addition, the false recognition rates under the condition of goal priming were lower than under the condition of the forget instruction, which might be interpreted as evidence for effective stereotype control. Implications of the outcome for stereotype control are discussed.

keywords
stereotype control, memory illusion, goal priming, forget instruction, 고정관념, 목표점화, 망각지시, 오기억

참고문헌

1.

서병숙, 김수현 (1999). 대학생의 노인에 대한 이미지연구. 한국노년학, 19, 97-111.

2.

이영숙, 박경란 (2003). 대학생이 인지하는 남, 여 노인의 고정관념 비교연구. 노인복지연구, 19, 83-109.

3.

한정란 (2000). 대학생들의 노인에 대한 태도에 관한연구. 한국노년학, 20, 115-127.

4.

Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits asknowledge structures: Automacity ingoal-directed behavior. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 78, 53-63.

5.

Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Holland, R. W. (2007). Thenonconscious cessation of goal pursuit: Whengoals and negative affect are coactivated.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,92, 165-178.

6.

Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Veltkamp, M. (2008). Goalpriming and the affective-motivational route tononconscious goal pursuit. Social Cognition, 26,555-577.

7.

Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice.Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

8.

Anaki, D., Faran, Y., Ben-Shalom, D., & Henik, A.(2005). The false memory and the mirror effects:The role of familiarity and backward associationin creating recollections. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 52, 87-102.

9.

Araya, T., Ekehammar, Bo., & Akrami, N. (2003).Remembering things that never occurred: Theeffects of to-be-forgotten stereotypicalinformation. Experimental Psychology, 50, 27-32.

10.

Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automaticstereotyping. Psychological Science, 7, 136-141.

11.

Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996).Automacity of social behavior: direct effects oftrait construct and stereotype activation onaction. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 71, 230-244.

12.

Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A.,Barndollar, K., & Trotschel, R. (2001). Theautomated will: Nonconscious activation andpursuit of behavioral goals. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 81,1014-1027.

13.

Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Gargano, G. J.(2003). Directed forgetting in implicit and explicitmemory tests: A comparison of methods. Journalof experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,and Cognition, 19, 603-616.

14.

Blair, I., & Banaji, M. (1996). Automatic andcontrolled processes in stereotype priming.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70, 1142-1163.

15.

Bloom. H. (1987). William Blake's The marriage ofHeaven and Hell. New York: Chelsea House Publishers

16.

Chartrand, T. L., Huber, J., Shiv, B., & Tanner, R.(2008). Nonconscious goals and consumer choice.Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 189-201.

17.

Clusters, R., & Aarts, H. (2007). Goal-discrepantsituations prime and goal-directed actions ifgoals are temporarily or chronically accessible.Personality and social psychology bulletin, 33,623-633.

18.

Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence ofparticular verbal intrusions in immediate recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17-22.

19.

Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and Prejudice:Their automatic and controlled components.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,56, 5-18.

20.

Devine, P. G., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M.,Harmon-Jones E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). Theregulation of explicit and implicit race bias: Therole of motivations to respond without prejudice.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,82, 835-848.

21.

Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., vanBaaren, R. B. (2006). On making the rightchoice: The deliberation-without-attention effect.Science, 311, 1005-1007.

22.

Dunning, D, & Sherman, D. A. (1997). Stereotypesand tacit inference. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 73, 459-471.

23.

Ferguson, M. J. (2007). On the automatic evaluationof end-states. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 92, 596-611.

24.

Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005).Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41,220-239.

25.

Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2009). Motions ofthe hand expose the partial and parallelactivation of stereotypes. Psychological Science,20, 1183-1188.

26.

Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble ofthinking: Activation and application of stereotypicbeliefs. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 60, 509-517.

27.

Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008). Implicitmotivation to control prejudice. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 44(1), 164-172.

28.

Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989).Gains and losses in development throughadulthood as perceived by different adult agegroups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109-121.

29.

Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Goals as knowledgestructures. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh(Eds.), Psychology of action: Linking conditionand motivation to behavior (pp.599-619). NewYork: Guilford Press.

30.

Lenton, A. P., Blair, I. V., & Hastie, R. (2001).Illusions of gender: Stereotypes evoke falsememories. Journal of Experimental SocialPsychology, 37, 3-14.

31.

Liberman, N., & Förstner, J. (2000). Expression aftersuppression: A motivational explanation ofpostsuppressional rebound. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 79, 190-203.

32.

Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V.(1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: Apeek inside the cognitive tool box. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 66, 37-47.

33.

Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., &Wheeler, V. (1996). On resisting the temptationfor simplification: Counterintentional effects ofstereotype suppression on social memory. SocialCognition, 14, 1-20.

34.

Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., &Ford, R. L. (1997). On regulation of recollection:The intentional forgetting of stereotypicalmemories. Journal of Personality and socialPsychology, 72, 709-719.

35.

MacLeod, C. M. (1999). The item and list methodsof directed forgetting: Test differences and therole of demand characteristics. PsychonomicBulletin and Review, 6, 123-129.

36.

Moscowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P., Wasel, W., &Schaal, B. (1999). Preconscious control ofstereotype activation through chronic egalitariangoals on attention. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 77, 167-184.

37.

Moscowitz, G. B. (2002). Preconscious effects oftemporary goals on attention. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 38, 397-404.

38.

Moscowitz, G. B., & Gesundheit, Y. (2009). Goalpriming. In G. B. Moscowitz, & H. Grant (Eds.),The psychology of goals (pp.203-233). New York:The Guilford Press.

39.

Moscowitz, G. B., & Ignarri, C. (2009). Implicitvolition and stereotype control. European View ofSocial Psychology, 20, 97-145.

40.

Moscowitz, G. B., & Li, P. (2011). Egalitarian goalstrigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form ofstereotype control. Journal of ExperimentalSocial Psychology, 47, 103-116.

41.

Park, S. H., Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008).Implicit motivation to control prejudice moderatesthe effect of cognitive depletion on unintendeddiscrimination. Social Cognition, 26, 401-419.

42.

Perdue, C. W., & Gurtman, M. B. (1990). Evidencefor the automaticity of ageism. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 26, 199-216.

43.

Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995).Creating false memories: Remembering wordsnot presented in lists. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,21, 803-814.

44.

Roediger, H. L., III, Watson, J. M., McDermott, K.B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determinefalse recall: A multiple regression analysi Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21, 803-814.

45.

Sela, A., & Shiv, B. (2009). Unraveling priming:When does the same prime activate a goalversus a trait? Journal of Consumer Research,36, 418-433.

46.

Shah, J. Y. (2003). Automatic for the people: Howrepresentations of significant others implicitlyaffect goal pursuit. Journal of Personality andsocial Psychology, 84, 661-681.

47.

Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Whenopportunity knocks: Bottom-up priming of goalsby means and its effect on self-regulation.Journal of Personality and social Psychology, 84,1109-1122.

48.

Shah, J. Y. (2005). The automatic pursuit andmanagement of goals. Current directions inpsychological science, 14, 10-13.

49.

Steffens, M. C., & Mecklenbräuker, S. (2007). Falsememories: Phenomena, theories, and implications.Journal of Psychology, 215, 12-34.

50.

Tse, C. S., & Neely, J. H. (2005). Assessingactivation without source monitoring in the DRMfalse memory paradigm. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 53, 532-550.

51.

Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mentalcontrol. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52

한국심리학회지: 사회 및 성격